The third weakness of the CKT concerns counter-
measures—that is, things that a deceptive person
might do in an effort to defeat or distort the test.
Research shows the CKT to be susceptible to mental
and physical countermeasures if subjects are knowl-
edgeable about the technique and have received train-
ing in the use of countermeasures.
AApppplliiccaattiioonn ooff tthhee CCKKTT
Although a great deal is written about the CKT in
the scientific literature, it presently has very little
application in either law enforcement or national
security. There is essentially no application of the
CKT in the United States. The only country that
reports a general use of the CKT in law enforcement
is Japan. In Japan, persons with special training in
psychology and eyewitness memory are part of the
crime scene investigation team, and they actively
search for and document possible bits of information
for use in CKT when the crime scene is first investi-
gated. It may be that this careful crime scene docu-
mentation results in a higher rate of applicability for
the technique. However, a clear explanation of how
Japanese examiners overcome the memorability prob-
lem is not presently in evidence.
The Deception Approach
TThhee TTeecchhnniiqquueess
The Relevant-Irrelevant Test. The deception approach
asks direct accusatory questions (referred to as
Relevant questions) under the assumption that persons
attempting deception will produce physiological
responses when they lie. The earliest version of the
deception approach was the Relevant-Irrelevant Test
(RIT). Along with direct accusatory questions (e.g.,
Did you shoot John Doe?), the RIT also asks irrele-
vant (neutral) questions, to which the person is
assumed to be responding truthfully (e.g., Are the
lights on in this room?). The working assumption of
the RIT is that persons attempting deception will pro-
duce a large and consistent physiological response to
the relevant questions, whereas the truthful will not
distinguish between the irrelevant and the relevant
questions.
Virtually all the scientists who work in this area
dismiss the working assumptions of the RIT as naive.
Clearly the truthful will recognize the relevant ques-
tions as the more important class of stimuli and are
thus likely to produce physiological responses to
them, and in fact, research does show a very large
number of false-positive outcomes to the RIT. As a
result, the RIT has very little application in forensic
polygraph testing. However, the RIT is still in use for
periodic screening of sex offenders and in screening
job applicants. At this time, any use of the RIT is
highly controversial, and the scientists active in this
area do not support its use.
Comparison Question Tests.John Reid developed the
notion of an active comparison question in the context
of law enforcement examinations during the late 1940s
in response to the obvious problems with the RIT. The
idea of the active comparison question was to provide
a stimulus in the test that would evoke physiological
responses from the innocent but not from the guilty.
The comparison question took the form of a question
that the subject was probably going to respond to with
a lie. For example, after discussing the death of John
Doe and after the subject of the examination has
denied being involved in John Doe’s death, the poly-
graph examiner would tell the subject that he or she is
going to be asked some questions about his or her
basic character in an effort to show that he or she is not
the type of person who would have shot John Doe. The
subject would then be asked a question such as
“Before the year 2006, did you ever hurt someone?”
The comparison question is deliberately vague and
covers a long period of time. In the context of the
examination, the subject is led to believe that an affir-
mative response is damaging because it shows that he
or she is the kind of person who would have commit-
ted the crime. However, for virtually all subjects, it can
be assumed that a definitive “No” response is probably
a lie in view of the deliberately vague presentation of
the comparison question.
The working assumption of the Comparison
Question Test (CQT) is that guilty participants will
produce consistent physiological responses to the
relevant questions, while they will respond only
minimally to the comparison questions. Although the
guilty are assumed to be lying in their answers to the
comparison questions, it is assumed that the compar-
isons are likely to be viewed as unimportant compared
with the relevant questions, which directly address the
issues under investigation. The innocent are expected
to respond more to the comparison questions because
they know that they are lying or are at least uncertain
about the veracity of their answers to the comparison
questions, whereas they know they are responding
598 ———Polygraph and Polygraph Techniques
P-Cutler (Encyc)-45463.qxd 11/18/2007 12:43 PM Page 598