Encyclopedia of Psychology and Law

(lily) #1
occurs independent of the content of the questions; in
fact, just asking a witness to think repeatedly about an
event can lead to later increases in witness confidence.
Such increases in confidence are problematic because
trial jurors place a great deal of weight on witness
confidence when judging the accuracy of a witness’s
testimony.
Repeated recall of portions of an event can lead to
retrieval-induced forgetting.Many studies in the cog-
nitive literature, some using eyewitness memory para-
digms, have demonstrated that the act of recalling
certain information about an event can actually impair
a person’s performance on a future memory task for
other previously unretrieved items. That is, repeated
questioning about some details of an event (e.g., the
male robber in a bank heist) may make it more diffi-
cult for the witness to recall other details about the
robbery later on (e.g., the female robber who was
waiting outside). Because interviews of eyewitnesses
conducted by the police, investigators, and attorneys
often constitute incomplete retrieval tasks, repeated
questioning of witnesses can lead to retrieval-induced
forgetting that may impair recall later at trial.
Finally, it should be noted that young witnesses,
particularly children, are generally more suggestible
than adult witnesses, and they are especially suscepti-
ble to all the effects of repeated recall discussed here.
In addition, some studies have shown that children
will change their answers to questions that are asked
multiple times, simply because they assume that their
original answers must have been wrong.

John S. Shaw III

See alsoCognitive Interview; Eyewitness Memory; Postevent
Information and Eyewitness Memory

Further Readings
MacLeod, M. (2002). Retrieval-induced forgetting in
eyewitness memory: Forgetting as a consequence of
remembering.Applied Cognitive Psychology,
16,135–149.
Shaw, J. S., III, & McClure, K. A. (1996). Repeated
postevent questioning can lead to elevated levels of
eyewitness confidence. Law and Human Behavior,
20,629–653.
Zaragoza, M. S., & Mitchell, K. J. (1996).
Repeated exposure to suggestion and the creation
of false memories.Psychological Science,
7,294–300.

REPORTINGCRIMES


ANDVICTIMIZATION


Almost all crimes become known to the police because
citizens, usually victims, report them. In this role as
gatekeeper, victims weigh their concerns about injus-
tice, their own security, and the security of the commu-
nity against the costs of reporting the crime, which may
include a belief that the police are unlikely to arrest the
offender or return stolen property and the notion that
involvement in the criminal justice process is time-con-
suming and possibly humiliating. Typically, victims are
more likely to report more serious crimes. The infor-
mation and advice that victims solicit and receive from
others are, often, also important. Just as in the case of
victims, bystanders’ decisions to report criminal events
are frequently subject to the social influence of others.
In the United States, there are more than 23 million
crimes annually—more than 18 million property crimes
and more than 5 million violent crimes. Since 1993,
rates of crime have generally been declining, in part due
to changing demographics (i.e., fewer people in the
most crime-prone ages) but also due to the decline in the
use of some drugs and to improved law enforcement
practices. Although victims are stereotyped as being dis-
proportionately female, White, and older, in fact, there is
an overlap between offenders and victims, such that
both offenders and victims are disproportionately male,
Black, and young. Males are more likely to be victim-
ized by a stranger, whereas females are more likely to be
victimized by someone they know.

Absolute Reporting Rates

Reporting is generally considered by psychologists to
be a type of help-seeking behavior. Most victims of
crime seek help from others, although, depending on
the crime, most do not call the police. Typically, vic-
tims seek help from family and friends, and some may
also seek help from mental health professionals.
Less than half of all crime is reported to the police—
about 47% of violent crimes and about 40% of property
crimes. The biggest single predictor of whether a crime
will be reported is the severity of the offense. About
60% of aggravated assaults, about 40% of simple
assaults, about 55% of burglaries, about 80% of motor
vehicle thefts, and about 30% of thefts are reported to
the police. More generally, reporting is more likely for

Reporting Crimes and Victimization——— 685

R-Cutler (Encyc)-45463.qxd 11/18/2007 12:43 PM Page 685

Free download pdf