Encyclopedia of Psychology and Law

(lily) #1
knowledge. For example, an episode may consist of ini-
tiating events that led the defendant to a certain mental
state, which then led the defendant to create goals or to
have certain motivations, which then resulted in the
defendant acting in a particular way. Jurors may get
some of the information to complete the episode from
trial evidence and may have to infer some of the episode
elements based on their knowledge of the world.
Episodes are hierarchically organized, in that each com-
ponent of any given episode may be broken down into
its own individual episode. The highest level episodes
are the most important in explaining the actions that
occurred; thus, the story model accounts for the weight
jurors assign to different pieces of evidence.
One of the greatest strengths of the story model is
that it accounts for jurors’ unique experiences and indi-
cates when they add those unique experiences and
pieces of knowledge into the decision-making process.
Witnesses do not often have the opportunity to explain
why particular events happened or how they personally
reacted to a particular sequence of events. So, jurors fill
in those blanks with inferences based on their own per-
sonal knowledge of similar events. In addition, jurors’
expectations about what makes a complete story are
important to help them determine when important
pieces of information are missing or when an inference
about human behavior or how someone might act in a
particular situation needs to be made. Jurors then can
make this inference based on their personal knowledge.
The story model accounts for differences in stories
between jurors by accounting for the different world
experiences and expectations jurors bring to the story
construction process.

CCeerrttaaiinnttyy PPrriinncciipplleess
Three certainty principles govern whether a juror
will find a particular story acceptable, and following
acceptability, how much confidence a juror will have
in a particular story. The first two—coverage and coher-
ence—contribute to whether a story will be accepted
and, if accepted, how much confidence the juror will
have in the story. The third principle, uniqueness, con-
tributes solely to the juror’s confidence in the story.
Coverage refers to the amount of evidence
accounted for by a particular story. The more coverage
a story has, the more likely the juror is to deem the story
acceptable and, if accepted, the more confidence the
juror will have in that story. Conversely, the less evi-
dence the story accounts for, the less likely it is to be

accepted by jurors. If accepted, jurors are likely to have
less confidence in the story compared with if the story
had a high level of coverage. If a story has low cover-
age and, therefore, the juror has a low amount of confi-
dence in the story, the juror would also have a low level
of confidence in the final decision based on that story.
The second principle is coherence. A story’s coher-
ence is determined by a combination of three variables:
consistency, plausibility, and completeness. To be con-
sistent, a story must contain no internal contradictions
or contradictions with pieces of evidence the juror
believes are true. To be plausible, the story constructed
must be similar to the juror’s knowledge of what typi-
cally happens in these situations. That is, a story must
not contradict the juror’s knowledge about the world in
general. Last, to be complete, the story must contain all
the parts of what a juror believes makes up a story. The
more consistent, plausible, and complete the story, the
higher the coherence of the story. If a story is deemed
coherent, the juror is more likely to think that the story
is an acceptable account of the events in question and
the more likely the juror is to be confident in the story.
The story model posits that jurors can construct
more than one story, but one story usually emerges as
the best explanation of events. However, what hap-
pens when jurors create more than one story and each
of the stories is high in both coverage and coherence?
According to the story model, this compromises the
last certainty principle, the uniqueness of the story. If
jurors construct more than one acceptable story, they
are less likely to believe either story, and the confi-
dence in both stories goes down.

Learning Verdict Options

In the second stage of the story model, jurors learn
which verdict options are available to them. Generally,
the different verdict options along with the definition
of what constitutes each verdict option are given to the
jurors in the judicial instructions at the end of the trial.
The combination of this one-trial learning task with the
difficulty of legal language makes this task difficult for
jurors. In fact, research has demonstrated that jurors
generally do not comprehend the majority of judicial
instructions presented to them. In addition, jurors also
have preconceived notions of what constitutes various
crimes. Those preconceived notions, accurate or inac-
curate, may interfere with jurors’ understandings of the
verdict options in the case.

Story Model for Juror Decision Making——— 767

S-Cutler (Encyc)-45463.qxd 11/18/2007 12:44 PM Page 767

Free download pdf