Voices may easily be disguised, further decreasing
the ability of a witness to accurately recognize a
voice. When a witness hears a voice raised in anger
during the commission of a crime and subsequently
attempts to recognize the speaker saying something in
a normal tone, accuracy is decreased. Whispering is
an extremely effective way to disguise a voice,
because it covers up many distinctive vocal character-
istics such as pitch.
Earwitness accuracy may also be related to the age of
the witness. Studies tend to show that very young
children are not as accurate in recognizing voices as
children above 10 years of age, who often perform com-
parably with adults. Speaker identification accuracy also
decreases after the age of 40, probably related to
increases in hearing loss for older persons. Furthermore,
blind persons are not superior to sighted persons in their
ability to recognize voices or other natural sounds, in
spite of popular opinion to the contrary.
Common sense tells us that recognizing the voice of
an acquaintance, friend, or family member should be
easier than recognizing the voice of a stranger. To a cer-
tain extent, research supports this conclusion. However,
studies of the recognition of familiar voices find a wide
range of accuracy levels, depending on the specific cir-
cumstances of the event. Although some studies find a
high degree of accuracy (more than 95%) in recogniz-
ing familiar voices, studies often show accuracy rates of
less than 70%, and sometimes significantly lower.
Daniel Yarmey and colleagues, for example, compared
participants’ recognition of highly familiar voices
(immediate family members or best friends), moder-
ately familiar voices (co-workers, teammates, or
friends), or low-familiarity voices (casual acquain-
tances) and found that accuracy for identifying voices
of low and moderate familiarity was only about 65%
and participants misidentified the voices of strangers as
being familiar almost 40% of the time. Thus, according
to Yarmey, when a witness claims to recognize a perpe-
trator’s voice as that of a familiar person, police officers
should not simply take this statement at face value but
should construct a voice lineup to test the witness’s
ability to identify the voice in question.
Unfortunately, the most salient indicator of voice
recognition accuracy for a juror is often the witness’s
confidence in the courtroom. Studies consistently
show that voice identification accuracy is almost com-
pletely unrelated to confidence. Extremely confident
witnesses are often wrong in their identification of a
voice, and accurate witnesses often show little confi-
dence in their identifications. Furthermore, jurors are
likely to overestimate the likelihood of any voice
identification being accurate. When psychology
students, for example, are asked to estimate the per-
centage of accurate identifications in circumstances
mirroring actual laboratory and field studies, they
consistently give unrealistically high accuracy predic-
tions. While it may not be surprising that laypersons
have little knowledge of the problems associated with
earwitness identification, a recent British study indi-
cated that police officers were no more knowledge-
able than the general population regarding voice
recognition issues.
Voice Identification Procedures
In another parallel with eyewitness research, the use of
one-person showups in voice identification has been
criticized as unduly suggestive. In a study by Daniel
Yarmey and his associates, a young woman approached
citizens individually in a public place and interacted
with them for about 15 seconds each. The participants
were given a voice identification test approximately
5 minutes after the encounter. When the test was a one-
person showup as opposed to a lineup to six voices,
innocent suspects were significantly more likely to be
identified. Accurate identifications of the real speaker’s
voice were rare in both conditions.
Within the United States and, for the most part,
internationally, there are few standardized procedures
for use in forensic voice identification. Researchers at
the Netherlands Forensic Institute have proposed the
development of guidelines for voice lineup construc-
tion, similar to the guidelines in use in many police
departments for eyewitness lineups. They advocate a
minimum of five voices in the lineup in addition to the
suspect, with foils being chosen for similarity to the
suspect’s sex, age, accent, socioeconomic background,
and vocal characteristics, such as pitch and speed of
speaking. They also recommend the use of double-
blind administrators and standardized instructions for
the earwitness—recommendations that are becoming
common in the realm of eyewitness procedure but need
stronger advocacy in the voice recognition arena.
Lori R. Van Wallendael
See alsoConfidence in Identifications; Estimator and System
Variables in Eyewitness Identification; Eyewitness
Identification: Field Studies; Eyewitness Memory;
Eyewitness Memory, Lay Beliefs About; Juries and
Eyewitnesses; Retention Interval and Eyewitness
Memory; Showups; Weapon Focus
854 ———Voice Recognition
V-Cutler (Encyc)-45463.qxd 11/18/2007 12:44 PM Page 854