Cognitive Science and the New Testament A New Approach to Early Christian Research

(Axel Boer) #1

Testament, there are various indications that they were highly esteemed
among some of thefirst Christians (cf. Vaage & Wimbush, 1999): for example,
John the Baptist lived an ascetic life in the desert (Mk 1:4–6); Jesus himself is
said to have spent forty days in the desert (Mk 1:12); poverty is an advantage
and richness is a disadvantage when it comes to entering the Kingdom of
Heaven (Luke 6:20; Matt. 19:16–30); Paul“punishes”and“enslaves”his body
(1 Cor. 9:27); Christians are warned“to abstain fromfleshly lusts, which
war against the soul”(1 Pet. 2:11). These and similar references in the New
Testament suggest that stressful conditions could influence the early Christian
cultural transmission and thought-world. An influence of this sort can be seen
in the frequent use of some emotionally laden subjects, such as the crucified
Lord, the Kingdom of Heaven, or thefinal judgment, as well as in the emphasis
on visionary language and apocalyptic imagery in most writings of the
New Testament.
Self-relatednessis yet another factor that influences the formation of mem-
ories in rituals. A number of empirical studies have shown that items related to
the self are remembered better than items not related to the self (C. S. Symons &
Johnson, 1997; Cloutier & Macrae, 2008). For example, subjects in an experi-
ment better remembered words that they thought to describe them well, than
words without such self-reference (Rogers et al., 1977). Recently it has been
found that self-relatedness rather than self-reference is enough to enhance
memory. In an experiment (Cloutier & Macrae, 2008), participants alternately
selected numbers that the experimenter matched with a list of words, uttering
the word that was linked with the number. Participants better remembered the
words linked to numbers they selected than words linked to numbers that
another subject selected. Thisfinding is related to the well-documented obser-
vation that ownership (even if it is artificially induced in an experiment) makes
things special, valuable, and attractive. The experiment demonstrates that
actions (and circumstances of actions) of which we are agents are remembered
better than the actions of others, as well as their results are monitored and
memorized better than the results of other people’s actions. The memory
effects of self-relatedness might have two different implications for memory in
rituals. First, people might form stronger memories of rituals that they undergo
voluntarily than of rituals to which they are subjected by the rules of their
societies. For example, all else being equal, the details of voluntarily chosen
baptism or an initiation to a mystery are probably better remembered than
obligatory initiation rites marking adulthood. Second, if an individual sees
himself or herself as the agent of a ritual, he or she might remember it better
than an individual who is the recipient of a ritual, such as the initiate of an
initiation rite. Learning might also enhance self-relatedness: for example, learn-
ing about the significance of baptism or another initiation rite for oneself might
enhance its self-relatedness and therefore its memorability. Paul provides rich
theological interpretation for baptism and emphasizes its personal nature in


Ritual 113
Free download pdf