78 Scientific American, September 2019 Graphic by Jen Christiansen
SOURCE:
RISK: A VERY SHORT INTRODUCTION,
BY BARUCH FISCHHOFF AND JOHN K ADVANY. OXFORD UNIVERSITY PRESS, 2011;
REDRAFTED FROM “HOW SAFE IS SAFE ENOUGH? A PSYCHOMETRIC STUDY OF ATTITUDES TOWARDS TECHNOLOGICAL RISKS AND BENEFITS,” BY BARUCH FISCHHOFF ET AL., IN
POLICY SCIENCES,
VOL. 9, NO. 2; APRIL 1978
mate research. Sponsored by the Department of En-
ergy, the project had five working groups. One dealt
with the oceans and polar regions, a second with the
managed biosphere, a third with the less managed
biosphere, and a fourth with economics and geo-
politics. The fifth group, which I joined, dealt with so-
cial and institutional responses to the threat.
Even then, 40 years ago, the evidence was strong
enough to reveal the enormous gamble being taken
with our planet. Our overall report, summarizing all
five groups, concluded that “the probable outcome is
beyond human experience.”
THINKING OF THE UNTHINKABLE
HOW, THEN, CAN RESEARCHERS in this area fulfill their duty
to inform people about accurate ways to think about
events and choices that are beyond their experience?
Scientists can, in fact, accomplish this if they follow
two basic lessons from studies of de cision-making.
LESSON 1: The facts of climate science will not speak
for themselves. The science needs to be translated
into terms that are relevant to people’s decisions
about their lives, their communities and their society.
While most scientists are experienced communicators
in a classroom, out in the world they may not get feed-
back on how clear or relevant their messages are.
Addressing this feedback problem is straight for-
ward: test messages before sending them. One can
learn a lot simply by asking people to read and para-
phrase a message. When communication researchers
have asked for such rephrasing about weather fore-
casts, for example, they have found that some are con-
fused by the statement that there is a “70 percent
chance of rain.” The problem is with the words, not
the number. Does the forecast mean it will rain 70 per-
cent of the time? Over 70 percent of the area? Or there
is a 70 percent chance of at least 0.01 inch of rain at
the weather station? The last interpretation is the cor-
rect answer.
Many studies have found that numbers, such as
70 percent, generally communicate much better than
“verbal quantifiers,” such as “likely,” “some” or “of-
ten.” One classic case from the 1950s involves a U.S.
National Intelligence Estimate that said that “an at-
tack on Yugoslavia in 1951 should be considered a se-
rious possibility.” When asked what probability they
had in mind, the analysts who signed the document
gave a wide range of numbers, from 20 to 80 percent.
(The Soviets did not invade.)
Sometimes people want to know more than the
probability of rain or war when they make decisions.
They want to understand the processes that lead to
those probabilities: how things work. Studies have
found that some critical aspects of climate change re-
search are not intuitive for many people, such as how
scientists can bicker yet still agree about the threat of
climate change or how carbon dioxide is diff erent
from other pollutants. (It stays in the at mosphere lon-
ger.) People may reject the research re sults unless sci-
entists tell them more about how they were derived.
LESSON 2: People who agree on the facts can still
disagree on what to do about them. A solution that
seems sound to some can seem too costly or unfair
to others.
For example, people who like plans for carbon cap-
ture and sequestration, because it keeps carbon diox -
ide out of the air, might oppose using it on coal-fired
power plants. They fear an indirect consequence:
cleaner coal may make mountaintop-removal mining
more acceptable. Those who know what cap-and-
trade schemes are meant to do—create incentives for
reducing emissions—might still believe that they will
benefit banks more than the environment.
These examples show why two-way communication
Pesticides
Nuclear power
Food coloring
Food preservatives
Spray cans
X-rays
Antibiotics
Contraceptives
Vaccination
Railroads
Electric
power
Home appliances
Football
Power mowers Bicycles
Alcoholic
beverages
Skiing
Swimming
Mountain climbing
Hunting
àyï ́
Police work Motorcycles
Smoking
Motor vehicles
Construction Handguns
General aviation
Surgery
y ́ïy ̈ĂDïD ̈j
́åï ̈ ̈màyDm
%¹ïmy ́ïy ̈ĂDïD ̈j
`¹®®¹ ́
́ÿ¹ ̈ù ́ïDàĂyāȹåùàyj
ù ́D® ̈Dàj
́yĀj
`DïDåïà¹È`ȹïy ́ïD ̈
<¹ ̈ù ́ïDàĂyāȹåùàyj
D® ̈Dàj
¹ ̈mj
`๠́`àå§
Risky Business
The way people view îxßä¦ä ̧
îx`³ ̧§ ̧xäD³lD`îþîxälxÇx³lä ̧³
D`
î ̧ßääø`Dä
D§DßîājÿxîxßxĀÇ ̧äøßxäþ ̧§ø³îDßā ̧ß³þ ̧§ø³îDßājD³lîx
§¦x§ ̧ ̧l ̧
DîD§îxäÍ% ̧þx§îāj³þ ̧§ø³îDßāxĀÇ ̧äøßxD³l§xîD§Ç ̧îx³îD§§xDl
Çx ̧ǧxî ̧ßDîxî³äDäßä¦xßjDääxääx³îäîDîä ̧xîxälxß
ß ̧ä`x³î
`` ̧ø³îäD³lxäîDîxäÍ5xßxäø§îä` ̧x
ß ̧äøßþxāäþx³î ̧§DāÇx ̧ǧxD³l
ßäîÇøU§äxl³¿ ́è}çîxāDþxUxx³ßxÇxDîxl ̧
îx³ÿîä§Dß³l³äÍ