The Wall Street Journal - 17.08.2019 - 18.08.2019

(Sean Pound) #1

THE WALL STREET JOURNAL. **** Saturday/Sunday, August 17 - 18, 2019 |C9


BYMIKEJAY


C


OCA-COLA, we are of-
ten reminded, used to
contain cocaine. Less
frequently do we recall
that the recipe for 7 Up
once included lithium. Just as Coca-
Cola was part of a late-19th-century
vogue for stimulant drinks, 7 Up was
one of many products, including lith-
ium waters and lithium beers, pro-
moted in the early 20th century as
a calming tonic. Lithia Springs, Ga.,
became a booming resort on the
strength of its lithium-rich waters:
A direct train from New York trans-
ported the wealthy, including several
presidents—Cleveland, McKinley,
Roosevelt and Taft—to its spas.
Lithium, a silvery-white metallic
element, is abundant in natural
sources, including rocks and sea-
water. It is highly reactive, however,
and was only discovered in its
pure form in 1817. By the time of
the lithium health craze, lithium
had established industrial uses in
ceramics, glassmaking and lubri-
cants. Its electroconductive prop-
erties have since made it an impor-
tant component of batteries and
cellphones.
In 1949 an Australian psychiatrist
named John Cade made the curious
discovery that lithium was extremely
effective in treating mania. As the
psychiatrist Walter Brown writes in
his thorough and highly readable
“Lithium: A Doctor, a Drug, and a
Breakthrough,” this was “the first
demonstration that a drug can alle-
viate the fundamental symptoms of
a mental illness.” It was followed by
a succession of psychotropic drugs
that transformed the landscape of
psychiatry—which until then had
been dominated by psychoanalysis—
into the pharmaceutically driven
profession of today.
Yet lithium’s story is not the tri-
umphant march of an irresistible
new wonder drug. It was promptly
abandoned by its discoverer and
largely ignored by the psychiatrists,
despite increasingly persuasive evi-
dence of its efficacy. This was partly
due to bad timing and partly to
problems with dosage and toxicity.
But the major obstacle was that
lithium, as an element, could not be
patented, a factor that continues to
limit its use today.
Cade was a provincial doctor
with, as Dr. Brown puts it, an “incli-
nation to thoroughly examine the
world around him” and “an aptitude
for taking in the unforeseen.” His


early papers considered the role of
vitamins and nutrition in disease,
a phenomenon he observed during
World War II while interned by the
Japanese in Singapore’s notorious
Changi Prison.
Cade’s study of deficiency dis-
eases such as pellagra and hypo-
thyroidism convinced him that some
mental illnesses had a biological
basis. By collecting urine samples
from patients in the mental hospi-
tal where he worked and injecting
the samples into guinea pigs, he
established that the urine of those
patients diagnosed with mania was
particularly toxic. Following a
rationale that remains somewhat
murky, he focused his research on
lithium salts. He began by testing

safe dosage levels on himself and
subsequently on patients in his hos-
pital. The first patient, whom he
referred to as “W.B.,” had been in a
chronic state of mania for several
years, but after a course of lithium
treatment was able to leave the
hospital and resume his old job.
Despite this spectacular early
success, Cade’s discovery bore no
further fruit. The vogue for lith-
ium tonics had already extended to
lithium compounds being used as a
substitute for table salt in dosages
that proved toxic and led to lithium
being banned from beverages by
the U.S. Food and Drug Adminis-
tration. Dosage was also a problem
for Cade’s research, as the drug
accumulated in the body during ex-

tended use. In 1950 W.B. developed
seizures, became comatose and even-
tually died from lithium poisoning.
Cade never mentioned this in his
subsequent papers and lectures,
but he banned its use in his own
hospital.
His work was soon picked up by
Mogens Schou. In 1954 the Danish
psychiatrist tested lithium in ran-
domized controlled trials, the first
to be carried out in psychiatry,
which confirmed the drug’s remark-
able efficacy. Schou established a
safe dosage level and a blood-serum
test to monitor it, and made the
further discovery that lithium not
only relieved the symptoms of mania
but acted as a prophylactic against
its recurrence.

Mind in Motion


By Barbara Tversky


Basic, 375 pages, $32


BYIANGROUND


H


OW ARE we to think
of how we think? Are
our minds a separate
internal world in
which we manipulate
mere proxies—symbols, ideas, repre-
sentations—for real things? Are they
software running in the brain whose
connection to the real, “external”
world is then a further mystery in
need of explanation? Or is it rather
that we are embodied all the way
down
, such that even our most
abstract thoughts—about mathe-
matics, say, or relations between
ideas—are still creatures of our crea-
turely nature?
In “Mind in Motion,” the distin-
guished cognitive scientist Barbara
Tversky makes the case that our em-
bodiment as living, acting creatures
is no mere add-on to our problem-
solving cognitive capacities. Rather,
she argues, the fact that we live and
move and act in a physical world
is fundamental, in very particular
ways, to the very nature of our
thought. Moving beyond theory, she
draws lessons about how to design
education, our environment and our
problem-solving strategies to help
us to live and think better.
Connoisseurs of TED talks and
popular books about cognitive sci-
ence will find some familiar exam-


ples here, including research about
cognitive biases, the patchy struc-
tures of attention, the active charac-
ter of sensory perception and so on.
There are also provocative discus-
sions of comic books, calendars and
cartography, and lessons from artists
and writers of instruction manuals.
Though these have much intrinsic
interest, Ms. Tversky’s examples are
more persuasive when they draw on
her own research on, for example,
our navigational abilities. She has
shown that we happily make sense
of perspectives that mix our own
(“egocentric”) position—turn right
and then left—with a neutral (“allo-
centric”) reference frame—turn
north, then west: a result that un-
dermines a presupposition in psy-
chology and linguistics that “we
need a uniform, coherent perspec-
tive not only to talk and understand
but also to think.”
Ms. Tversky’s prose is energetic
and muscular, constantly moving to
make new connections. In one of
several lists of verbs that we use to
describe our thinking, she writes
that “we arrange and rearrange,
enlarge, stretch, reverse, join, copy,
add, scramble, subtract, lift, glue,
push, fold, mix, toss, embellish, sep-
arate, nail, scatter, bury, eliminate,
turn, elevate, and poke holes in both
real objects and mental ones.” This
has its own kind of poetry, almost
enough to quiet a worry about why
everyday talk about our cognitive
processes—what is sometimes called
“folk psychology”—should lead to
hard conclusions about our cog-
nition. Of course, it is of interest
that we use action and spatial meta-
phors to characterize thought. But
we might wonder whether there
is any well of metaphor we could
draw from that is not somehow con-
nected with the ways we exist in the
physical world.

Ms. Tversky succeeds in assem-
bling from a vast range of sources
plenty of evidence for something
within touching distance of her cen-
tral claim. There is an interesting
discussion of research on how land-
marks anchor our judgments of dis-
tance. We are inclined, for example,
to think that the distance between
Jack’s apartment and the Empire
State Building is shorter than the
distance between the Empire State
Building and Jack’s apartment.
Yet the argument seems some-
times to lightly dance from one meta-
phor to another, as stepping stones
across deep waters. Ms. Tversky
avoids bringing to bear the more
general theoretical and philosophical
context for her work. Thus she makes
only passing reference to the intellec-
tual movements that have shaped the
history of the cognitive sciences, and
none to those that have for some
years been reshaping broader philo-
sophical conceptions of mind.
Furthermore, there remains some
mystery at the heart of the book
about the relationship between its
key concepts. The central concept is
action—though it is not quite clear
how this is distinguished from mere

that are still latent in the cogni-
tive sciences. Mind is conceived of
as an engine for processing exclu-
sively internal representations—
what were once called “ideas”—with
Ms. Tversky adding the notion that
this processing is shaped by our
having bodies and moving in space
and time. This is rather like saying,
to adapt a remark by the philoso-
pher Mary Midgley, that the inside
of the teapot is shaped by the out-
side. Overcoming that unnecessary
division may require more radical
reorientations in our thinking.
There are some tantalizing signs
that Ms. Tversky understands the
conventional picture needs rethink-
ing. “The mind is too small,” she
writes at one point; “the world has
far more space.” That is, we don’t
process a separate internal model of
the world but actively use the world
itself to do cognitive work: texts,
pictures, gestures, arrangements of
objects and so on. But she quickly
reverts to the more mainstream
thought that we use the world to do
the work only when our cognitive ca-
pacities are overloaded. The trouble
is that this contention is quite differ-
ent from claiming that the world and
our actions are implicated in the
very possibility of thinking.
Ms. Tversky has written an im-
pressive, passionate and mildly he-
retical romp through cognitive sci-
ence. Not long ago, it was considered
intellectually sophisticated to believe
that while computers might one day
think, dogs don’t. We humans are,
like other thinking creatures, cogni-
tive animals, and not merely soggy
computers. Every reminder of that
fact deserves, like this high-spirited
account, to be warmly recommended.

Mr. Ground is a research fellow
in philosophy at the University of
Hertfordshire.

No Ideas


But


In Things


GETTY IMAGES

movement. For much of the book,
however, she focuses on how our
cognition is informed by the spatial.
It is true that we act or move in
space, but does that mean that ac-
tions shape thought or that space
does? Even then, dealing with the
spatial, the emphasis is almost en-

tirely on the visual, with only pass-
ing references to touch: what is
called the “haptic.” We are left to
wonder whether our haptic judg-
ments, about the center of gravity of
an object we hold in our hands, say,
are structured by space or the visual
or bodily activity.
Perhaps part of the problem here
is that Ms. Tversky has not com-
pletely freed her thinking from the
oversharp divisions between mind
and body, representation and world,

The fact that our brains
are in bodies shapes
how they think. We are
cognitive animals, not
merely soggy computers.

Lithium


By Walter A. Brown


Liveright, 222 pages, $27.95


Schou’s attempts to establish lith-
ium therapy was met with resistance
from within the psychiatric profes-
sion. A 1968 Lancet article suggested
it was “another therapeutic myth”
being pushed aggressively by a small
cadre of “enthusiastic advocates.”
Dr. Brown, who at the time was be-
ginning his own psychiatric train-
ing at Yale, recalls that he “was
instructed to treat depressed pa-
tients in line with the prevailing psy-
choanalytic formulation” and “was
encouraged to avoid or minimize the
use of psychiatric drugs.”
Larger and more rigorously de-
signed trials eventually swung the
balance of medical opinion toward
lithium. Yet no pharmaceutical com-
pany filed a new-drug application
with the FDA until 1970, at which

point the FDA approved its medical
use. Uptake remained slow, especially
in the U.S., where it was undercut in
the 1980s by the arrival of valproate,
a compound with no great thera-
peutic advantages but one that could
be patented and that the pharma-
ceutical industry promoted heavily.
Today, lithium inhabits a psychi-
atric world that is in many ways
the opposite of the one in which it
emerged. Psychoanalysis has given
way to biomedicine, and lithium
competes in a teeming marketplace
of medications. Yet it remains an
anomaly. In Dr. Brown’s assessment,
it is “uniquely specific” in its action,
“effective only in manic-depressive
illness” (now commonly known as
bipolar disorder). But despite its
chemical simplicity, we still have
no idea how it works. Nor has it
revealed a biological cause for bi-
polar disorders.
At the same time, recent research
has validated the old idea that small
quantities of lithium are broadly
beneficial to health. It appears to
preserve and foster the growth of
nerve cells, and areas where the
water has higher lithium levels show
lower suicide rates. It seems at small
doses to be a general mental tonic,
much as advertised during the last
century by the “lithiated lemon-lime
soda” sold as 7 Up.

Mr. Jay is the author, most
recently, of “Mescaline: A Global
History of the First Psychedelic.”

A Mysterious Balm for Mania


UP AND DOWN John Cade in 1974. The Australian psychiatrist recognized lithium’s medical potential.


NEWSPIX

The naturally occurring
element is effective in
treating bipolar disorder.
But scientists still have
no idea how it works.

BOOKS


‘It’s terrible to think that all I’ve suffered and all the suffering I’ve caused might have arisen from the lack of a little salt in my brain.’—ROBERT LOWELL

Free download pdf