Writing for Research

(Jeff_L) #1
Raewyn Connell

Summative writing


This is the writing that gets your findings into other people’s hands. It’s the subject of
most “Advice about Writing” texts, which teach you how to write journal articles, theses
or books. Other forms of summative writing exist, such as the old-fashioned
palaeontology monograph describing a particular species of fossil arthropod. (There’s a
very entertaining account of that genre in chapter 3 of Stephen Jay Gould’s book
Wonderful Life .) I suspect new forms of summative writing are emerging on the Internet.


For the time being, as I noted earlier, the journal article is queen. Therefore Part Two of
this booklet will detail how to write a journal article. Here, I want to raise a general issue
about summative writing.


Summative writing always involves selection; in fact, severe selection. Even with a long
book – I’ve written a few – the research produces far more material than the author can
include. Be kind to the reader! A journal usually forces selection by having a maximum
word length for articles. It is painful to leave out rich illustrations, amusing quotes, and
elegant arguments; but it has to be done. Get used to this pain, it will be with you
throughout a research career.


Selective interpretation 1966
(Image: Australian Liberal Party, from
The War Room, Nichols ed.)

A familiar point in the advice texts: good
writing is defined as much by what it
leaves out, as by what it puts in. The
problem in writing for research is to
select in a way that doesn’t distort the
truths established by the research.
Selective interpretation, as much as
stating falsehoods, is the rule in
propaganda.

Even without propaganda intent,
selection can mean distortion. Because
researchers and journals prefer
statistically significant results (there’s a
thrill in writing p<.01), many findings of
“no significant difference” go unreported
or unnoticed. I am convinced that this
pressure has distorted the whole field of
social-science and psychology “sex
difference” research. It really ought to be
called “sex similarity” research, because
p>.05 (i.e. no significant difference) is the
most common finding.
Free download pdf