Advances in Role and Reference Grammar

(singke) #1

258 WILLIAM H. JACOBSEN, JR.


conjunction reduction, relationships among nexus types, directionality of
clause chaining, and generalized clause chaining.

13.1 Frequency of subordination

Making my best judgements about clause boundaries (and counting only
clauses), I find the proportion of subordinate clauses (to the total of inde­
pendent, absolutive, and subordinate clauses) to be 13.5% in the narrative
passages of our sample and 9.2% in the direct quotes. These figures are a
little higher than the lows of 2% to 7% reported by Mithun (section 1), but
still much less than the 28% for Tlingit and the 34% to 46% for English.
This is striking, given the full development of Nootka devices for explicit
subordination, which obviate the need for some of the indirect approaches
outlined by Mithun (1984:497-505). The factor of polysynthesis that she
adduced applies here also, of course. But probably also relevant is the
occurrence of cosubordination, given that 57.4% of the clauses in the narra­
tive text are absolutive. These cosubordinated clauses doubtless relieve the
pressure to avoid repeating all the clausal operators of combined clauses
that leads to more frequent subordination in other languages.^19 Jeffers
(1976) considers Old Irish, Ancient Egyptian, Squamish, and Tahitian, and
concludes that VSO languages will be sparse in non-finite verbal forms
(participles, gerunds, infinitives) and will favor embedding by nominaliza-
tion. This fits Nootka fairly well: the absolutive and subordinate forms are
finite in that they inflect for subject, and otherwise subordination is by core
nominalization.^20

13.2 Cosubordination vs. conjunction reduction

In an important paper, Kiparsky (1968) discussed the early Indo-European
injunctive verb forms, which are best attested in Vedic Sanskrit. These are
forms with "secondary" endings but lacking the augment (a prefix *e-)
elsewhere associated with them. Kiparsky shows that these forms, like the
Nootka absolutive, neutralize the verbal categories of tense and mood,
expressing only person, number, and voice. They occur commonly con­
joined to non-injunctive forms, whose tense and mood they are understood
as also expressing. Their other main types of occurrence are with the pro­
hibitive adverb ma, and also — again like the Nootka absolutive — uncon-
joined in ritual and mythological passages. Kiparsky also demonstrates that
Free download pdf