Invitation to Psychology

(Barry) #1

212 Chapter 6 Sensation and Perception


signals through touch and internal sensations,
memories of these signals remain in the nervous
system, including memories of pain, paralysis, and
cramping that occurred prior to amputation. The
result is an inaccurate “body map” in the brain and
pain signals that cannot be shut off.
Vilayanur Ramachandran, the neurologist
who first proposed this explanation of phantom
pain, has developed an extraordinarily simple
but effective treatment for it. Ramachandran
wondered whether he could devise an illusion to
trick the brain of an amputee with phantom arm
pain into perceiving the missing limb as mov-
ing and pain-free. He placed a mirror upright
and perpendicular to the sufferer’s body, such
that the amputee’s intact arm was reflected in
the mirror. From the amputee’s perspective, the
result was an illusion of two functioning arms.
The amputee was then instructed to move both
arms in synchrony while looking into the mir-
ror. With this technique, which has now been
used with many people, the brain is fooled into
thinking its owner has two healthy arms or legs,
resynchronizes the signals—and phantom pain
diminishes or even vanishes (Ramachandran &
Altschuler, 2009). Neurologists have tested this
method with Iraq veterans and are finding it
to be more successful than control therapies in
which patients just mentally visualize having two
intact limbs (Anderson-Barnes et al., 2009; Chan
et al., 2007).

pain can help explain the many instances of severe,
chronic pain that occur without any sign of injury
or disease whatsoever.

Input from
spinal cord

Memories
Emotions
Expectations
Attention

Signals from
brain centers

Abnormal patterns
of activity in a
neural matrix

Pain

Because technology now allows scientists to
study pain at the molecular and cellular level, to-
day we know that the gate-control theory, though
still useful, is incomplete. Pain may also be caused
by changes to the sensitivity of neurons in the
central nervous system (CNS). For example, when
bursts of heat are repeatedly applied to an animal’s
paw, pain receptors in the paw and skin remain
sensitive for several hours. During that time, even
a light, harmless touch will activate the recep-
tors, and applying anesthetic at the site of injury
will not make the receptors return to normal.
Such changes can help explain how once-harmless
stimuli can end up causing pain (Latremoliere &
Woolf, 2009).
An extreme version of pain without injury oc-
curs in phantom pain, in which a person continues
to feel pain that seemingly comes from an arm
or leg that has been amputated, or from a bodily
organ that has been surgically removed. Phantom
limb pain afflicts up to 90 percent of amputees.
The person may feel the same aching, burning, or
sharp pain from sores, calf cramps, throbbing toes,
or even ingrown toenails that he or she endured
before the surgery. Even when the spinal cord has
been completely severed, amputees often continue
to report phantom pain from areas below the
break. There are no nerve impulses for the spinal-
cord gate to block or let through, yet the pain can
be constant and excruciating; some sufferers have
committed suicide.
A leading explanation of phantom pain is
that the brain has reorganized itself: An area in
the sensory cortex that formerly corresponded
to the missing body part has been “invaded” by
neurons from another area. Higher brain centers
then interpret messages from those neurons as
coming from the nonexistent body part (Cruz
et al., 2005; Ramachandran & Blakeslee, 1998).
Even though the missing limb can no longer send

phantom pain The
experience of pain in a
missing limb or other
body part.


After his right leg was destroyed in an explosion while
he was in Iraq, Army Sergeant Nicholas Paupore experi-
enced excruciating phantom limb pain. Even morphine
didn’t help. Then he underwent a simple daily proce-
dure. A mirror was placed at a strategic angle to reflect
his intact leg, tricking his brain into registering two
healthy legs that he could move freely. The pain sub-
sided almost immediately. A year after therapy, he had
only occasional, milder pain, and needed no medication.
In some patients, mirror therapy has eliminated phantom
pain entirely.
Free download pdf