374 ChapteR 10 Behavior in Social and Cultural Context
These paintings done during wartime poignantly illustrate one child’s effort to portray the horror of war and another’s
dream of peace. Can we learn to design a world in which conflicts and group differences, though inevitable, need not
lead to violence?
Taking Psychology With You
The compelling evidence for the banality of evil
is difficult for many people to accept. Clearly, some
people do stand out as being unusually heroic or
unusually sadistic. But in the social-psychological
view, evil does not result from aggressive instincts but
from the all-too-normal processes we have discussed
in this chapter, including ethnocentrism, obedience
to authority, conformity, groupthink, deindividuation,
stereotyping, and prejudice.
The good news is that when circumstances within
a nation change, societies can also change from being
warlike to being peaceful. Ethnocentrism and prejudice,
along with compassion, cooperation, and altruism, are
part of our human heritage, awaiting the conditions that
will awaken them. By identifying the conditions that
create the banality of evil, perhaps we can create other
conditions that foster the “banality of virtue”—everyday
acts of kindness, selflessness, and generosity.
bloodless hands. Americans and Canadians slaugh-
tered native peoples in North America, Turks slaugh-
tered Armenians, the Khmer Rouge slaughtered
millions of fellow Cambodians, the Spanish conquis-
tadors slaughtered native peoples in Mexico and South
America, Idi Amin waged a reign of terror against his
own people in Uganda,
the Japanese slaugh-
tered Koreans and
Chinese, Iraqis slaugh-
tered Kurds, despotic
political regimes in
Argentina and Chile
killed thousands of dissidents and rebels, the Hutu
in Rwanda murdered thousands of Tutsi, and in the
former Yugoslavia, Bosnian Serbs massacred thou-
sands of Bosnian Muslims in the name of “ethnic
cleansing.”
Thinking
CriTiCally
About “Evil” Cultures
Dealing With Cultural
Differences
A French salesman worked for a company
that was bought by Americans. When the
new American manager ordered him to step
up his sales within the next three months,
the employee quit in a huff, taking his cus-
tomers with him. Why? In France, it takes
years to develop customers; in family-owned
businesses, relationships with customers
may span generations. The American man-
ager wanted instant results, as Americans
often do, but the French salesman knew this
was impossible and quit. The American view
was, “He wasn’t up to the job and disloyal
besides, so he stole my customers.” The
French view was, “There is no point in ex-
plaining anything to a person who is so stu-
pid as to think you can acquire loyal custom-
ers in three months” (Hall & Hall, 1987).
Both men were committing the funda-
mental attribution error: assuming that the
other person’s behavior was due to his per-
sonality rather than the situation, in this
case one governed by cultural rules. Such
rules are not trivial and success in a global
economy depends on understanding them.
But you don’t have to go to another country
to encounter cultural differences; they are
right where you live.
If you find yourself getting angry over
something a person from another culture
is doing, use the skills of critical thinking
to find out whether your expectations and
perceptions of that person’s behavior are
appropriate. Take the time to examine your
assumptions and biases, consider other ex-
planations of the other person’s actions,
and avoid emotional reasoning. For example,
people who shake hands as a gesture of
friendship and courtesy are likely to feel
insulted if a person from a non-hand-shaking
culture refuses to do the same, unless they