Invitation to Psychology

(Barry) #1

448 ChapTER 13 Emotion, Stress, and Health


an individual, also serve important functions:
appeasing others when you feel you have made
a fool of yourself, broken a moral rule, or vio­
lated a social norm (Dijk, de Jong, & Peters,
2009; Feinberg, Willer, & Keltner, 2012). And
the positive emotions of joy, love, laughter, and
playfulness do not appear to be simply self­
ish feelings of pleasure; their adaptive function
may be to help increase mental flexibility and
resilience, build bonds with others, stimulate
creativity, and reduce stress (Baas, De Dreu, &
Nijstad, 2008; Kok, Catalino, & Frederickson,
2008).
The components of emotion are physiological
changes in the face, brain, and body; cognitive
processes such as interpretations of events; action
tendencies that spur us to fight or flee, embrace or
withdraw; and subjective feelings. In turn, culture
and social context influence both the inner expe­
rience and the outward expression of emotion.
If we compare human emotions to a sculpture,
the biological capacity for emotion is the clay;
thoughts create its size, shape, and details; and
culture judges the result, placing some sculp­
tures front and center and relegating others to
the attic.

Emotion and the Body LO 13.2
Everywhere on the planet, people feel certain
basic emotions, including fear, anger, sadness,
joy, surprise, disgust, shame, embarrassment, and
contempt (Izard, 2007; Keltner & Buswell, 1997).
These emotions have distinctive physiological
patterns and corresponding facial expressions,
but they and others are also evoked by common
human experiences: All over the world, sadness
follows perception of loss, fear follows percep­
tion of threat and bodily harm, anger follows
perception of insult or injustice, and so forth
(Scherer, 1997).
Let’s begin with some findings from neuro­
scientists and others who are studying the biologi­
cal aspects of emotions: facial expressions, brain
regions and circuits, and the autonomic nervous
system.

The Face of Emotion. The most obvious place
to look for emotion is on the face, where emo­
tions are often visibly expressed. In 1872, Charles
Darwin argued that human facial expressions—
the smile, the frown, the grimace, the glare—are
as innate as the wing flutter of a frightened bird,
the purr of a contented cat, and the snarl of a
threatened wolf. Such expressions evolved, he

emotion A state of
arousal involving facial
and bodily changes,
brain activation, cogni-
tive appraisals, subjective
feelings, and tendencies
toward action.


said, because they allowed our ancestors to tell
at a glance the difference between a friendly
stranger and a hostile one, prepared our forbears
to respond to challenges in the environment, and
enabled them to communicate important infor­
mation to others.
Modern psychologists have supported Dar­
win’s ideas about the evolutionary functions of
emotion (Hess & Thibault, 2009; Shariff & Tracy,
2011). Years ago, Paul Ekman and his colleagues
gathered abundant evidence for the universal­
ity of the facial expressions of seven emotions:
anger, happiness, fear, surprise, disgust, sadness,
and contempt (Ekman, 2003; Ekman et al., 1987).
In every culture they studied—in Brazil, Chile,
Estonia, Germany, Greece, Hong Kong, Italy,
Japan, New Guinea, Scotland, Sumatra, Turkey,
and the United States—a large majority of people
recognized the emotional expressions portrayed
by those in other cultures (see Figure 13.1). Even
most members of isolated tribes who had never
watched a movie or read People magazine, such
as the Foré of New Guinea or the Minangkabau
of West Sumatra, could recognize the emotions
expressed in pictures of people who are entirely
foreign to them, and Westerners could recognize
theirs. Some researchers have added pride to the
list, arguing that its adaptive function is to moti­
vate people to achieve and excel, and thereby
to increase their attractiveness to others and to
their groups (Tracy & Robins, 2008; Williams &
DeSteno, 2009).
Simulate the Experiment Recognizing Facial
Expressions of Emotions at MyPsychLab
Ekman and his associates developed a coding
system to analyze and identify each of the nearly
80 muscles of the face, as well as the combinations
of muscles associated with each emotion (Ekman,
2003). When people try to hide their feelings and
display a false emotion, they generally use differ­
ent groups of muscles than they do for authentic
ones. If they try to pretend that they feel sad, only
15 percent manage to get the eyebrows, eyelids,
and forehead wrinkle exactly right, as they would
if they were expressing true grief spontaneously.
Authentic smiles last only 2 seconds; false smiles
may last 10 seconds or more (Ekman, Friesen, &
O’Sullivan, 1988).

The Functions of Facial Expressions. Inter­
estingly, facial expressions not only reflect our
internal feelings but also influence them. In the
process of facial feedback, the facial muscles send
messages to the brain about the basic emotion

facial feedback The
process by which the
facial muscles send mes-
sages to the brain about
the basic emotion being
expressed.

Free download pdf