458 ChapTER 13 Emotion, Stress, and Health
categories of perception are suspended—at twilight,
in the brush, watching fires glow without heat
(Levy, 1984). Englishspeaking people can identify
a bittersweet experience or feel the mixed emotions
of pleasure and regret in nostalgia. But English lacks
an emotion word that is central to inhabitants of the
tiny Micronesian atoll of Ifaluk: fago, translated as
“compassion/love/sadness,” which reflects the sad
feeling one has when a loved one is absent or in
need, and the pleasurable sense of compassion in be
ing able to care and help (Lutz, 1988).
Do these interesting linguistic differences
mean that Germans are more likely than others
to actually feel schadenfreude, the Japanese to feel
hagaii, Tahitians to feel mehameha, and English
speakers to feel nostalgia? Or are they just more
willing to give these blended emotions a single
name? Most people in all cultures are apparently
capable of feeling the emotions that have distinc
tive physiological hallmarks in the brain, face, and
nervous system. But people in different cultures
might indeed differ in their ability to experience
emotional blends and variations.
In Chapter 7, we noted that a prototype is a
typical representative of a class of things. People
everywhere consider some emotions to be pro
totypical examples of the concept emotion: Most
people will say that anger and sadness are more
representative of an emotion than irritability and
nostalgia are. Prototypical emotions are reflected
in the emotion words that young children learn
first: happy, sad, mad, and scared. But as children
develop, they begin to draw emotional distinc
tions that are less prototypical and more specific
to their language and culture, such as ecstatic,
depressed, hostile, or anxious (Hupka, Lenton, &
Hutchison, 1999; Shaver, Wu, & Schwartz, 1992).
In this way, they come to experience the nuances
of emotional feeling that their cultures emphasize.
Culture also influences the causes of emotion
and shapes their expression. Anger may be universal,
but the way it is experienced will vary from culture
to culture—whether it feels good or bad, useful or
destructive. And cultures determine much of what
people feel emotional about. For example, disgust
is universal, but the content of what produces dis
gust changes as an infant matures, and it varies
across cultures (Pole, 2013; Rozin, Lowery, & Ebert,
1994). People in some cultures learn to become
disgusted by bugs (which other people find beauti
ful or tasty), unfamiliar sexual practices, dirt, death,
“contamination” by a handshake with a stranger, or
particular foods (e.g., meat if they are vegetarian;
pork if they are Muslims or Orthodox Jews).
Communicating Emotions
Suppose that someone dear to you died. Would
you cry, and if so, would you do it alone or in
public? Your answer will depend in part on your
culture’s display rules for emotion (Ekman et al.,
1987; Gross, 1998). In some cultures, grief is
expressed by weeping; in others, by tearless res
ignation; and in still others by dance, drink, and
song. Once you feel an emotion, how you express
it is rarely a matter of “I say what I feel.” You may
be obliged to disguise what you feel. You may wish
you could feel what you say.
display rules Social
and cultural rules that
regulate when, how,
and where a person may
express (or suppress)
emotions.
Around the world, the cultural rules for expressing emotions differ. The display rule for
a formal Japanese wedding portrait is “no direct expressions of emotion,” but not every
member of this family has learned that rule yet.