Invitation to Psychology

(Barry) #1
Chapter 1 What Is Psychology? 33

Interpreting the Findings LO 1.20


The last step in any study is to figure out what the
findings mean. Trying to understand behavior from
uninterpreted findings is like trying to become flu-
ent in Swedish by reading a Swedish–English dic-
tionary. Just as you need the grammar of Swedish
to tell you how the words fit together, psycholo-
gists need hypotheses and theories to explain how
the facts that emerge from research fit together.


Choosing the Best Explanation. Sometimes
it is hard to choose between competing explana-
tions. Does cell phone use disrupt driving by
impairing coordination, by increasing a driver’s
vulnerability to distraction, by interfering with
the processing of information, by distorting the
driver’s perception of danger, or by some combi-
nation of these or other factors? Several explana-
tions may fit the results equally well, which means
that more research will be needed to determine
the best one.
Sometimes the best interpretation does not
emerge until a hypothesis has been tested in dif-
ferent ways. If the findings of studies using dif-
ferent methods converge, there is greater reason
to be confident about them. On the other hand,
if they conflict, researchers will know they must
modify their hypotheses or do more research.


True mean in the population

Repeated studies

Study 1
Study 2
Study 3
Study 4
Study 5


FIGURE 1.5 Confidence Intervals Across Repeated
Studies
If you repeat your study over and over, you will get a
slightly different sample mean and confidence inter-
val each time. But the vast majority of the confidence
intervals would contain the true population mean (the
straight vertical line). Occasionally, though, you will
get a rogue result (the gray bars). That is why it can be
risky to draw strong conclusions on the basis of only one
study: It might be the one rogue.


Cross-Sectional Study
Different groups compared at one time:
Group A (20-year-olds)
Group B (50-year-olds) compared
Group C (80-year-olds)

Longitudinal Study
Same group compared at different times:
Group A
at age 20

Group A
at age 50

Group A
at age 80

But longitudinal studies, in which the same
people are followed over a period of time and
reassessed at regular intervals, can also be used to
investigate mental abilities across the life span:

longitudinal study A
study in which people (or
animals) are followed and
periodically reassessed
over a period of time.

In contrast to cross-sectional studies, longitudi-
nal studies find that as people age, they sometimes
continue to perform as well as they ever did on
many mental tests. A general decline in ability may
not occur until people reach their 70s or 80s (see
Chapter 3). Why do results from the two types of
studies conflict? Probably because cross-sectional
studies measure generational differences. Younger
generations tend to outperform older ones in part
because they are better educated or more familiar
with the tests used. Without longitudinal studies,
we might falsely conclude that all types of mental
ability inevitably decline with advancing age.

Judging the Result’s Importance. Sometimes
psychologists agree on the reliability and meaning
of a finding but not on its ultimate relevance for
theory or practical application. Part of the problem
is statistical. Traditional tests of significance are
widely used in psychology, which is why we have
described them here, but these tests have some
serious drawbacks
(Cumming, 2012;
Cumming et al.,
2007; Erceg-Hurn &
Mirosevich, 2008). A
result may be statisti-
cally significant at the
“point oh-five level,”
yet be small and of little consequence in everyday
life because the independent variable does not
explain most of the variation in people’s behavior.

About “Significant”
Research Findings

Thinking
CriTiCally

Here is an example. When psychologists
compare the mental-test scores of young people
and old people, they usually find that younger
people consistently outscore older ones. This type
of study, in which different groups are compared
at the same time, is called a cross-sectional study:

cross-sectional study
A study in which groups
of people (or animals) of
different ages are com-
pared at a given time.
Free download pdf