Persuasive Communication - How Audiences Decide. 2nd Edition

(Marvins-Underground-K-12) #1

106 Understanding Rational Decision Making


group level is “the degree to which information, ideas, or cognitive processes are shared or are


being shared among the group members.”^11


The information-processing approach is also the leading theoretical approach to the emerging

fi eld of team cognition and decision making.^12 Much like a single individual, a team makes deci-


sions on the basis of internalized cognitive processes rather than automatically accepting any new


information presented to it.^13


Figure 3.3 represents a famous early model of a simple decision-making task developed by psy-

chologist Saul Sternberg that inspired many cognitive scientists to adopt the information-processing


approach.^14 The model predicts that people go through four elementary steps when deciding


whether a particular one-digit number (e.g., 7) is a part of a multiple-digit number (e.g., 472) they


had memorized earlier. First they perceive the one-digit number, then they compare it to each digit


in the multiple-digit number they had memorized, then they make their decision, and fi nally they


generate a response.


Tests of the model showed that people take each step in the model in sequence and indepen-

dently of the other steps. If the one-digit number were made to look blurry and hard to perceive,


only the time subjects took to complete the perception step was affected. If a digit were added to


the memorized multiple-digit number, subjects took an additional 38 milliseconds to complete


the comparison step but took no additional time to complete the other steps. If subjects had been


biased to decide yes or no before they saw the one-digit number, only the time taken to complete


the decision-making step was affected.


Outside the laboratory, audiences are less likely to take each subsequent step in the decision-making

process than the step before it. For example, the decision to comply with warning labels and signs


has been shown to be impeded because people rarely perceive the warnings in the fi rst place.^15


Only 24% of the swimmers at a high school pool they regularly used recalled seeing the conspicu-


ous “NO DIVING” sign next to it. Only 20% of the students in a home economics class recalled


seeing any information on an iron they regularly used for two weeks despite the fact the iron was


clearly labeled with a hazard warning.^16


Even if audience members do perceive information, they may not attend to and comprehend it.

In another study of warning labels, although 88% of the consumers in the study recalled seeing the


warning on the product, only 46% read even a portion of the warning.^17 Moreover, only 27% made


the decision to comply with the warning. Prior steps in the decision-making process can infl uence


subsequent steps in other ways as well. For example, faster recognition of letters and words predicts


better comprehension skills, whereas increases in comprehension do not predict increases in word


recognition.^18


The information-processing approach has several other important characteristics. It focuses on

the mental behaviors of individuals and views people as active, goal-oriented information proces-


sors, not as passive blank slates to be written upon. It acknowledges, for example, that audiences


STIMULUS PERCEPTION COMPARISON DECISION RESPONSE

Example: After the subject memorized the number 472, Sternberg asked, “Does the number
you memorized include 7?”

7 Perceive 7 = 4? 7 = 7? 7 = 2? Make Generate “Yes”
stimulus decision response

FIGURE 3.3 Sternberg’s Information-Processing Model of a Simple Decision

Free download pdf