Ancient Literacies

(Marvins-Underground-K-12) #1

‘‘A cat,’’ a child may make a scribble; if asked to write ‘‘Two cats,’’ they


may make two squiggles, and so on. But if asked to write ‘‘No cats,’’ they


may say, ‘‘I didn’t write anything because there are no cats.’’ Writing


requires sustained attention to the linguistic form as opposed to what


the language is about. To oversimplify somewhat, writing distinguishes


what is said from what is meant, capturing only the former. American


linguist Benjamin Lee Whorf (1956) suggested that we dissect nature


along lines laid down by our native language to show how our thought


tends to run in conventional linguistic ruts. I revised this claim to the


context of literacy to say, ‘‘We introspect our language along lines laid


down by our scripts’’ (Olson 1994, 90).


The literacy hypothesis, then, is the hypothesis that a writing system


and a tradition of writing is not a neutral practice; it allows us, indeed


invites us, to think about language and mind in some new ways. Eric


Havelock (1982) pioneered some of these ideas, claiming that the fixity of


text allowed writing to take over the mental functions previously carried


by memory. He traced some of the properties of Homeric texts to their


oral composition and contrasted that with the beginnings of written
poetry and especially written prose, views that have been importantly


elaborated and extended by Powell (2002). Certainly it remains an inter-


esting project to trace the ways that writing influenced discourse and the


specialization of genre in both speech and writing.


But it is also possible to examine how writing contributes to two very


specialized uses of language, one that may be described as the isolation of


‘‘pure thought’’ and the second as the elaboration of subjectivity. For


more than a century anthropologists and sociologists have associated


cultural development with two features of language use: the increase of


rational thought and rationalized practices on one hand and the growth


of subjectivity, the consciousness of the mental states, on the other.


My question, the central point of this chapter, is to ask how literacy


could contribute to these two special properties of social and cultural


evolution.


THOUGHTS WITHOUT THINKERS—LITERACY


AND ABSTRACT PROSE


The unit of thought that a sentence expresses is a proposition, the unit of


language first systematically analyzed by Frege (reprinted 1970) at the


end of the last century. One of Frege’s important contributions was to


show that a simple declarative sentence is actually composed of two parts,
an ‘‘assertoric force’’ and a ‘‘predicate.’’ Thus, an utterance, Frege argued,


does two things at once: it both mentions a thought, the predicate, and


asserts it as true, its assertoric force. Thus there are two things hidden


inside the utterance of an ordinary declarative sentence. His distinction


later provided the basis for Austin’s (1962) theory of speech acts (Lee


Why Literacy Matters, Then and Now 393

Free download pdf