Time - USA (2021-02-15)

(Antfer) #1

26 Time February 15/February 22, 2021


TheView Science


How dreams


defend our brains


By David Eagleman and Don Vaughn


What do brain flexibility and rapid
cortical takeover have to do with dream­
ing? Perhaps more than previously
thought. Ben clearly benefited from
the redistribution of his visual cortex
to other senses because he had perma­
nently lost his eyes, but what about the
participants in the blindfold experi­
ments? If our loss of a sense is only tem­
porary, then the rapid conquest of brain
territory may not be so helpful. And
this, we propose, is why we dream.
In the ceaseless competition for
brain territory, the visual system has a
unique problem: because of the planet’s
rotation, all animals are cast into dark­
ness for an average of 12 out of every
24 hours. Our ancestors effectively were
unwitting participants in the blindfold
experiment, every night of their lives.
So how did the visual cortex of our
ancestors’ brains defend its territory,
in the absence of input from the eyes?
We suggest that the brain preserves
the territory of the visual cortex by
keeping it active at night. In our “defen­
sive activation theory,” dream sleep ex­
ists to keep neurons in the visual cortex
active, thereby combatting a takeover
by the neighboring senses. In this view,
dreams are primarily visual precisely
because this is the only sense that is dis­
advantaged by darkness. Thus, only the
visual cortex is vulnerable in a way that
warrants internally generated activity
to preserve its territory.

In humans, sleep is punctuated by
rapid eye movement (REM) sleep every
90 minutes. This is when most dream­
ing occurs. REM sleep is triggered by
a specialized set of neurons that pump
activity straight into the brain’s visual
cortex, causing us to experience vision
even though our eyes are closed. This ac­
tivity in the visual cortex is presumably
why dreams are pictorial and filmic. The
anatomical precision of these circuits
suggests that dream sleep is biologically
important—such precise and universal
circuitry rarely evolves without an im­
portant function behind it.
The defensive activation theory
makes some clear predictions about
dreaming. For example, because brain
flexibility diminishes with age, the frac­
tion of sleep spent in REM should also
decrease across the life span. And that’s

When he Was 2 years old, Ben sTopped seeing ouT of
his left eye. His mother took him to the doctor and soon dis­
covered he had retinal cancer in both eyes. After chemo­
therapy and radiation failed, surgeons removed both his eyes.
For Ben, vision was gone forever.
But by the time he was 7 years old, he had devised a tech­
nique for decoding the world around him: he clicked with his
mouth and listened for the returning echoes. This method
enabled Ben to determine the locations of open doorways,
people, parked cars, garbage cans and so on. He was echo­
locating: bouncing his sound waves off objects in the environ­
ment and catching the reflections to build a mental model of
his surroundings.
Echolocation may sound like an improbable feat for a
human, but thousands of blind people have perfected this
skill, just as Ben did. How could blindness give rise to the
stunning ability to understand the surroundings with one’s
ears? The answer lies in a gift bestowed on the brain by
evolution: tremendous adaptability.
Whenever we learn something, pick up a new skill or mod­
ify our habits, the physical structure of our brain changes.
Neurons, the cells responsible for rapidly processing informa­
tion in the brain, are interconnected by the thousands—but
like friendships in a community, the connections between
them constantly change: strengthening, weakening and
finding new partners. Neuroscience calls this phenomenon
brain plasticity, referring to the ability of the brain, like plas­
tic, to assume new shapes and hold them. More recent discov­
eries in neuroscience, though, suggest that the brain’s brand
of flexibility is far more nuanced than holding onto a shape.
To capture this, we refer to the brain’s plasticity as “live­
wiring” to spotlight how this vast system of 86 billion neu­
rons and 0.2 quadrillion connections rewires itself every mo­
ment of your life. The brain’s livewiring allows for learning,
memory and the ability to develop new skills.
Recent decades have yielded several revelations about live­
wiring, but perhaps the biggest surprise is its rapidity. Brain
circuits reorganize not only in the newly blind but also in the
sighted who have temporary blindness. In one study, sighted
participants intensively learned how to read braille. Half the
participants were blindfolded throughout the experience.
At the end of five days, the participants who wore blindfolds
could distinguish subtle differences between braille charac­
ters much better than the participants who didn’t wear blind­
folds. Even more remarkably, the blindfolded participants
showed activation in visual brain regions in response to touch
and sound. In other words, the blindfolded participants per­
formed better on the touch­ related task because their visual
cortex had been recruited to help.
But such changes don’t have to take five days; that just hap­
pened to be when the measurement took place. When blind­
folded participants are continuously measured, touch­related
activity shows up in the visual cortex in about an hour.


26


YEARS


Amount of time
you will spend
asleep in an
average lifetime

21%


Proportion of
time sleeping
that we spend
dreaming

4 TO 6


CYCLES


Number of times
most people
experience a
dream sequence
every night

KANGHEE KIM

Free download pdf