Scientific American 201905

(Rick Simeone) #1

80 Scientific American, May 2019 Graphics by Tami Tolpa


FROM “NEURAL SUMMATION IN THE HAWKMOTH VISUAL SYSTEM EXTENDS THE LIMITS OF VISION
IN DIM LIGHT,” BY ANNA LISA STÖCKL ET AL., IN

CURRENT BIOLOGY,

VOL. 26, NO. 6; MARCH 21, 2016 (

flower images

)

times much more, depending on how he processed the images.
To test this effect on the beetles themselves, Foster built a
simplified, artificial Milky Way out of single-file LED lights
on an arch over an arena. He could vary the intensity of light
on each side. The beetles could go straight if he gave them a
13 percent contrast between one end of the bright line and
the other but wavered if the contrast dropped below that.
This result indicated the animals should be able to tell the
two ends of the real Milky Way apart.

SIGNAL BOOSTERS
in addition to beetles and bees, a number of other animals
are now known to see remarkably well in dark environments:
cockroaches, lantern fish, cuttlefish, frogs and nocturnal pri-
mates such as owl monkeys. So neuroscientists are turning to
the question of how they do it. Bigger eyes collect more light,
for example, but do not gather enough photons to explain
the highly sensitive night vision that scientists have docu-
mented. Other visual processing must take place after the
rods have absorbed incoming light. In particular, animals
must be able to overcome or filter out visual “noise” created
by photoreceptor activity that does not reveal anything use-
ful about the visible world.
Noise in the visual system comes from a few different sourc-
es. One, called photon shot noise, happens when only a few pho-
tons come into photoreceptors. Because those light packets
tend to arrive sporadically, they create a variable, unreliable pic-
ture. It’s as if you shone three or four flashlights around the ceil-
ing of the Sistine Chapel at night. You would hardly be able to
appreciate Michelangelo’s complete masterpiece.
A second source of noise arises from the molecular inter-
actions in the photoreceptors themselves. A photoreceptor
senses light when an incoming photon hits a molecule called
rhodopsin. But every so often—once a minute, at most—a
rhodopsin molecule is triggered by accident, or another part
of the pathway misfires. This is called dark noise because it
can happen even in pitch-black conditions with your eyes
closed. A third source, transducer noise, results from varia-
tion in the timing and strength of the visual system’s re -
sponse to a single real photon.
Noise isn’t a big problem in broad daylight, because the tre-
mendous volume of photons hitting the eyes overwhelms
these slight variations. In the dark, however, animals need a
strategy to boost the signal to similarly noise-overwhelming
levels. They do so by summing up the signals they get from
individual photoreceptors across space and time.
Spatial summation works like this: Imagine you are at a
concert where 1,000 fans are waving their illuminated cell
phones with excitement. You can’t see the light from each indi-
vidual phone all that well. If every group of 50 concertgoers
combined the light of their phones into a single, brighter spot-
light, you’d see those 20 spotlights really well. The retina—the
sheet of tissue that contains rods and cones—does the same,
pooling the input from numerous rods into a single, bigger sig-
nal that gets sent to the brain. At the concert, you lose the pic-
ture of each individual person waving a phone, and the same
thing happens in spatial summation; the resulting image is
brighter but also coarser.
Temporal summation also increases brightness. Rods slow

Time

Slow response

Day Night

Lamina
monopolar cell

Visual
unit
Pooled signal
Pooling lamina
monopolar cell

Spatial summing effect
in low light

Temporal summing effect
in low light

Fast response

Light signals

Nonpooled signal

Low-Light Image


Improvement


During the day, there is a lot of light for high-acuity vision. But
at night, the few available photons rarely stimulate photore-
ceptor cells and do so only weakly. The result is a grainy,
obscure image. Hawkmoths solve this problem by adding up
these scarce photons across both space and time.

Spatial Summation
In dim light, sparse image signals head from photoreceptors toward
visual-processing areas of the brain. But in some insects, in an intermediate
region, lamina monopolar cells pool together signals from adjacent spots
in the eye. This creates a brighter visual unit, but it loses sharpness because
it com bines signals from different locations.

Temporal Summation
Photoreceptor cells can slow down the rate at which they pass signals back
to the brain at night. Each individual signal, at fast speeds, is too weak to stim-
ulate much of a brain response in visual areas. But by slowing their responses
down, the cells let the signals pile on top of one another, creating a stronger
stimulus. This also improves brightness, though again with less acuity.

© 2019 Scientific American
Free download pdf