Child Development

(Frankie) #1

Teachers need to use a child’s name and articu-
late clearly and slowly when directing speech to a
hearing-impaired child. They should kneel at a desk
and focus on cuing. Cued speech, developed by Dr.
Cornett at Gallaudet University in Washington, D.C.,
is a system of teacher hand cues that enhances lip-
reading by children. The hand cues, used near the
lips, match what is being said to clarify ambiguities.
Cued speech used by a trained adult helps the hear-
ing-impaired school-age child increase reading skills.


Multimedia technology has been used to develop
materials and activities for Mexican-American deaf
children, the fastest-growing minority group in the
U.S. school-age population of deaf children. Video
dictionaries of sign language can be built directly into
stories onscreen. CD-ROM reading software is now
becoming available for hearing-impaired children.


Teachers and parents working together create
opportunities for deaf children so that they can use
total language communication to achieve inter-
personal goals. Enthusiasm and acceptance help
hearing-impaired children experiment with their lan-
guage systems so that they can learn well and have a
positive social effect on others, as well as engage in
peer friendships.


See also: AMERICAN SIGN LANGUAGE; LANGUAGE
DEVELOPMENT


Bibliography
Andrews, J. F., and D. L. Jordan. ‘‘Multimedia Stories for Deaf
Children.’’ Teaching Exceptional Children (May/June 1998):28–
33.
Blasi, M. J., and L. Priestley. ‘‘A Child with Severe Hearing Loss
Joins Our Learning Community.’’ Young Children 53, no. 2
(March 1998):44–49.
Gregory, S., and S. Barlow. ‘‘Interactions between Deaf Babies and
Their Deaf and Hearing Mothers.’’ In Bencie Woll ed., Lan-
guage Development and Sign Language. Bristol, Eng.: Interna-
tional Sign Linguistics Association, 1988.
Harris, John. Early Language Development: Implications for Clinical
and Educational Practice. London: Rutledge, 1990.
Harris, Margaret. Language Experience and Early Language Develop-
ment: From Input to Uptake. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum,
1992.
Kampfe, C. M., and A. G. Turecheck. ‘‘Reading Achievement of
Prelingually Deaf Students and Its Relationship to Parental
Method of Communication.’’ American Annals of the Deaf 132
(1987):11–15.
Klima, Edward S., and Ursula Bellugi. The Signs of Language. Cam-
bridge, MA: Harvard University Press, 1979.
Kovalik, Gail, Melanie Norton, and Susan Meck. Deafness: An Anno-
tated Bibliography and Guide to Basic Materials. Chicago: Ameri-
can Library Association, 1992.
Lederberg, A. R., and V. S. Everhart. ‘‘Conversations between Deaf
Children and Their Hearing Mothers: Pragmatic and Dialog-
ic Characteristics.’’ Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education 5
(2000):303–322.
Marschark, Marc. Raising and Educating a Deaf Child. New York:
Oxford University Press, 1997.


Mayne, A., C. Yoshinaga-Itano, A. L. Sedey, and A. Carey. ‘‘Expres-
sive Vocabulary Development of Infants and Toddlers Who
Are Deaf or Hard of Hearing.’’ Volta Review 100 (2000):1–28.
McArthur, Shirley H. Raising Your Hearing-Impaired Child: A Guide-
line for Parents. Washington, DC: Alexander Graham Bell As-
sociation for the Deaf, 1982.
Musselman, C., P. Lindsay, and A. Wilson. ‘‘An Evaluation of
Trends in Preschool Programming for Hearing-Impaired
Children.’’ Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders 53
(1988):71–88.
Quigley, Stephen P., and Cynthia M. King. ‘‘The Language Devel-
opment of Deaf Children and Youth.’’ In Sheldon Rosenberg
ed., Handbook of Applied Psycholinguistics: Major Thrusts of Re-
search and Theory. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum, 1982.
Quigley, S. P., and Peter V. Paul. Language and Deafness. London:
Croom Helm, 1984.
Thompson, Marie, P. Biro, S. Vethivelu, C. Pious, and N. Hatfield.
Language Assessment of Hearing Impaired School Age Children. Se-
attle: University of Washington Press, 1987.
Alice Sterling Honig

HEREDITY VERSUS ENVIRONMENT
Many aspects of human characteristics (such as height
and eye color) are largely genetically determined.
Psychology researchers, however, tend to be interest-
ed in dimensions that are relatively less determined
by genetics—traits that subject more to environmen-
tal influences, such as how a person feels, acts, and
thinks. Given that the degree of genetic determina-
tion appears to vary from one dimension to another
(e.g., spatial skills versus language acquisition), how
can one determine the relative influences of heredity
and environment for various human characteristics,
and how can one understand the complex relation-
ship between them?
For example, Javier has two biological daughters
who share the same biological mother. Both are tall,
well mannered, and musically inclined. Despite these
similarities, the older child appears socially reserved
and quiet, while the younger one, who was born into
the same family environment, seems more outgoing.
In addition, one of his children has been diagnosed
with a learning disability while the other seems excep-
tionally well-functioning cognitively. How can these
similarities and differences between the two children
be explained? One may think, ‘‘Well, Javier is tall and
he is also a talented musician himself, so these girls
must have gotten these ‘good genes’ from Javier. And
he is quite strict when it comes to disciplining his chil-
dren, so that explains their good manners. But why
is the younger one so sociable—and what about her
learning disability? Maybe she hasn’t been read to as
much as the older one has.’’ In essence, hereditary in-
fluences and various environmental factors in these
children’s lives are being weighed and analyzed in ex-
plaining the characteristics of these children.

HEREDITY VERSUS ENVIRONMENT 181
Free download pdf