Child Development

(Frankie) #1

Messer, Stephen C., Karl L. Wuensch, and John M. Diamond.
‘‘Former Latchkey Children: Personality and Academic Cor-
relates.’’ Journal of Genetic Psychology 150 (1988):301–309.
Mulhall, Peter F. ‘‘Home Alone: Is It a Risk Factor for Middle
School Youth and Drug Use?’’ Journal of Drug Education 26,
no. 1 (1996):39–48.
Posner, Jill K., and Deborah Lowe Vandell. ‘‘After-School Activities
and the Development of Low-Income Urban Children: A
Longitudinal Study.’’ Developmental Psychology 35 (1999):868–
879.
Quay, Lorene C. ‘‘Personal and Family Effects on Loneliness.’’
Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology 13, no. 1 (1992):97–
110.
Reynolds, Ruth C. ‘‘What Research Has to Say about Latchkey Pro-
grams.’’ ERIC Database no. ED267922 (1985).
Richardson, J. L., K. Dwyer, K. McGuigan, W. B. Hansen, C. Dent,
C. A. Johnson, S. Y. Sussman, B. Brannon, and B. Flay. ‘‘Sub-
stance Use among Eighth-Grade Students Who Take Care of
Themselves after School.’’ Pediatrics 84 (1989):556–565.
Rodman, Hyman, David J. Pratto, and Rosemary S. Nelson. ‘‘Child
Care Arrangements and Children’s Functioning: A Compari-
son of Self-Care and Adult-Care Children.’’ Developmental Psy-
chology 21 (1985):413–418.
Shafritz, Jay M., Richard P. Koeppe, and Elizabeth W. Soper. The
Facts on File Dictionary of Education. New York: Facts on File,
1988.
Steinberg, Lynne. ‘‘Latchkey Children and Susceptibility to Peer
Pressure: An Ecological Analysis.’’ Developmental Psychology 22
(1986):433–439.
U.S. Bureau of the Census. ‘‘All Parent/Child Situations, by Type,
Race, and Hispanic Origin of Householder or Reference Per-
son, 1970 to Present.’’ Washington, DC: U.S. Bureau of the
Census, 1998.
U.S. Bureau of the Census. ‘‘Married Couples by Labor for Status
of Spouse, 1986 to Present.’’ Washington, DC: U.S. Bureau of
the Census, 1998.
Vandell, Deborah Lowe, and Jill K. Posner. ‘‘Conceptualization
and Measurement of Children’s After-School Environments.’’
In Sarah L. Friedman and Theodore D. Wachs eds., Assessment
of the Environment across the Lifespan. Washington, DC: Ameri-
can Psychological Association Press, 1999.
Vandell, Deborah Lowe, and Janaki Ramanan. ‘‘Children of the
National Longitudinal Survey of Youth Choices in After-
School Care and Child Development.’’ Developmental Psycholo-
gy 27 (1991):637–643.
Woods, Merilyn B. ‘‘The Unsupervised Child of the Working
Mother.’’ Developmental Psychology 6 (1972):14–25.
Dennis H. Karpowitz


LEAD POISONING


Lead is an environmental toxin that can cause mental
retardation, brain damage, or death in children.
Young children are particularly at risk because they
can accidentally eat leaded paint chips or breathe
lead-contaminated dust. Although lead-based paint
for household use has been banned in the United
States since 1977, deteriorated older houses remain
important sources. Once inside the body, lead affects
the brain, heart, liver, kidney, and blood. Initially


only high measurable blood lead levels (<60 micro-
grams/deciliter or μg/dL) associated with seizures,
coma, or death were recognized as lead poisoning;
learning and behavior problems have been shown at
lower levels (10–20 μg/dL). Although national surveys
have shown decreases in blood lead levels in children
one to five years of age from 88 percent during 1976–
1980 to 4 percent during 1991–1994, poor young mi-
nority children in inner cities remain at risk for signif-
icant exposure. Treatment consists of eliminating
lead from the home environment, adding iron to the
diet, and, if necessary, providing medications to re-
move lead from the body.

See also: DEVELOPMENTAL DISABILITIES

Bibliography
American Academy of Pediatrics, Committee on Environmental
Health. ‘‘Screening for Elevated Blood Levels.’’ Pediatrics 101
(1998):1072–1078.
‘‘Blood Lead Levels in Young Children—United States and Select-
ed States, 1996–1999.’’ Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report 49
(2000):1133–1137.
Hwang, Mi Young, Richard Glass, and Jeff Molter. ‘‘JAMA Patient
Page: Protect Your Child Against Lead Poisoning.’’ Journal of
the American Medical Association 281 (1999):2406.
Markowitz, Morri. ‘‘Lead Poisoning.’’ Pediatrics in Review 21
(2000):327–335.
John I. Takayama

LEARNING
Learning can occur in a variety of manners. An organ-
ism can learn associations between events in their en-
vironment (classical or respondent conditioning),
learn based upon the reinforcements or punishments
that follow their behaviors (operant or instrumental
conditioning), and can also learn through observa-
tion of those around them (observational learning).
Learning principles are of particular importance for
school performance.

Classical or Respondent Conditioning
In the early twentieth century, Ivan Pavlov, a Rus-
sian scientist, unwittingly stumbled upon an impor-
tant discovery for the field of behavioral psychology.
While studying digestion in dogs, he discovered that
after being fed a few times, the animals would salivate
before actually receiving food. The dogs were associ-
ating external cues such as the sound of the food cabi-
net being opened with being fed, so they would
salivate upon hearing these sounds before they saw
the food.
This phenomenon is called classical conditioning
or respondent conditioning. Pavlov found that by

LEARNING 235
Free download pdf