Child Development

(Frankie) #1

produced far more manual babbling than three
matched hearing infants at similar ages. The deaf in-
fants’ hand babbling also revealed phonetic features
of American Sign Language, suggesting that babbling
reflects infants’ innate ability to analyze phonetic and
syllabic components of linguistic input.


Pre-Speech Productions and First Words


or Signs


Early words are produced by the child in expect-
ed contexts, and hence are recognized by familiar lis-
teners as linguistic units conveying meanings. In 1999
Esther Dromi distinguished between comprehensible
and meaningful words. Comprehensible words are
phonetically consistent forms resembling adult words
that caregivers understand, but that do not yet convey
referential meanings. Meaningful words are symbol-
ic, arbitrary, and agreed-upon terms of reference.
Considerable variation exists in both the age of
speech onset and the rate of early lexical develop-
ment. Large-scale questionnaire data reported in
1994 by Fenson and his colleagues for English-
speaking typically developing children, cited the
range of vocabulary size for twelve- to thirteen-
month-olds at 0 to 67 different words, and for eigh-
teen- to nineteen-month-olds at 13 to 471 different
words. In 2000 Maital and her colleagues reported
very similar figures for Hebrew.


Early words are constructed from a limited set of
consonants, mainly stops, nasals, and glides. Syllable
structures in these words are usually CV, CVC, or
CVCV. Several researchers found that during the first
few months of lexical learning, many new words are
composed from segments that the child is already
using in babbling. A number of researchers have pro-
posed that patterns of lexical selection and avoidance
reflect the child’s production capabilities. When pro-
ductive vocabularies contain more than a hundred
different words, the influences of phonology on the
lexicon decline. Nevertheless, children who have rela-
tively larger lexicons of single words also show larger
inventories of sounds and syllable structures than
children with smaller productive lexicons. Precocious
word learners have much larger phonetic inventories
than typically developing children at age eighteen
months. The major semantic achievement in the first
few months of vocabulary learning is the ability to use
words referentially. Martyn Barrett and Esther
Dromi, who independently carried out detailed longi-
tudinal analyses of repeated uses of the same words
over time, have argued that some early words show
referential use from their outset, while other words
are initially produced only in very specific contexts.
Throughout the one-word stage, the phonology of
words improves, and meanings become symbolic and


arbitrary. A word initially produced in just one situa-
tion is now uttered in a much wider range of contexts,
until it becomes completely context free and referen-
tial. As words become conventional tools for express-
ing meanings, the amount of pre-speech vocalizations
declines and gradually disappears.

See also: INFANCY; LANGUAGE DEVELOPMENT

Bibliography
Barrett, Martyn. ‘‘Early Semantic Representations and Early Word
Usage.’’ In Stan Kuczay and Martyn Barrett eds., The Develop-
ment of Word Meaning. New York: Springer, 1986.
Blake, Joanna, and Bénédicte de Boysson-Bardies. ‘‘Patterns in
Babbling: A Cross-Linguistic Study.’’ Journal of Child Language
19 (1992):51–74.
de Boysson-Bardies, Bénédicte. How Language Comes to Children:
From Birth to Two Years. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 1999.
Dromi, Esther. Early Lexical Development. Cambridge, Eng.: Cam-
bridge University Press, 1987.
Dromi, Esther. ‘‘Early Lexical Development.’’ In Martyn Barrett
ed., The Development of Language. London: UCL Press, 1999.
Fenson, Larry, Philip S. Dale, Steven J. Reznick, Elizabeth Bates,
Donna Thal, and S. J. Pethick. Variability in Early Communica-
tion Development. Malden, MA: Blackwell, 1994.
Jusczyk, Peter W. The Discovery of Spoken Language. Cambridge,
MA: MIT Press, 1997.
Locke, John L. The Child’s Path to Spoken Language. Cambridge,
MA: Harvard University Press, 1993.
Locke, John L. ‘‘Why Do Infants Begin to Talk? Language as an
Unintended Consequence.’’ Journal of Child Language 23
(1996):251–268.
Maital, Sharone L., Esther Dromi, Avi Sagi, and Marc H. Boren-
stein. ‘‘The Hebrew Communicative Development Inventory:
Language Specific Properties and Cross-Linguistic General-
izations.’’ Journal of Child Language 27 (2000):43–67.
Oller, Kimberly D., and Rebecca E. Eilers. ‘‘The Role of Audition
in Infant Babbling.’’ Journal of Child Language 59 (1988):441–
449.
Papouˇsek, Metchthild, and Hanus Papouˇsek. ‘‘Form and Function
of Vocal Matching in Interactions between Mothers and Their
Precanonical Infants.’’ First Language 9 (1989):137–158.
Petitto, Laura A., and Paula F. Marentette. ‘‘Babbling in the Manu-
al Mode: Evidence for the Ontogeny of Language.’’ Science
251 (1991):1493–1496.
Stoel-Gammon, Carol. ‘‘Role of Babbling and Phonology in Early
Linguistic Development.’’ In Amy M. Wetherby, Steven F.
Warren, and J. Reichle eds., Transitions in Prelinguistic Commu-
nication. Baltimore: Paul Brookes, 1997.
Veneziano, Edy. ‘‘Vocal-Verbal Interaction and the Construction of
Early Lexical Knowledge.’’ In M. D. Smith and John L. Locke
eds., The Emergent Lexicon: The Child’s Development of a Linguis-
tic Vocabulary. New York: Academic Press, 1988.
Vihman, Marilyn M. Phonological Development: The Origins of Lan-
guage in the Child. Cambridge, MA: Blackwell, 1996.
Esther Dromi

BANDURA, ALBERT (1925–)
Albert Bandura, who proposed the most comprehen-
sive and widely held theory of modeling, was born in

BANDURA, ALBERT 47
Free download pdf