Children\'s Mathematics

(Ann) #1
young learners. In the literature children are seen as needing to translate between
the informal and formal mathematical languages, though in practical terms there
has been a dearth of guidance on how this might be achieved. However, recent lit-
erature has very much supported the idea of older children using their own written
methods mathematics. For example, Thompson (1997) uses the term ‘personal
written methods’ and Anghileri (2000) writes about ‘invented methods’. There has
been lack of evidence from published examples of younger children’s mathematical
graphics, yet this is where most of the difficulty begins: children’s confusion is
ingrained before they might be given the freedom to try their own written ways in
mathematics. We are arguing that teachers need to understand children’s mathe-
matical marks much earlier than current available literature suggests.
We know the pressures that teachers experience, including tests, inspections and
demands for ‘results’ and ‘standards’. Some of these pressures can be interpreted as
a need for children to achieve abstract symbolism rather than use informal mathe-
matical graphics in the short term.The pressure leads to children being hurried on by
recording in ‘acceptable’ (neat or standardised) forms such as worksheets. It can also
include teachers’ requests that children use a particular form of representation: this
might include standard calculations, or that all children in the class use tallies or a
pictographic form when working on a particular calculation.
In Figure 5.2 this shift is represented as a bridge, with children moving fromtheir
informal mathematics to the standard and increasingly abstract forms of
representing mathematics in school. The implication in such a model is that the
desirable movement is generally of one-way traffic, therefore it will follow that the
most desirable position will be to move children on to abstract mathematics
without a transitional or multicompetentstage.
In Figure 5.3 we provide an alternative model. Using their own mathematical
graphics and constantly moving between their own informal understanding and
abstract mathematical symbolism in an infinite loop ensures cognitive feedback. In
the centre of the model the area represented by a dotted circle shows where

Figure 5.3 Becoming bi-numerate

80 Children’s Mathematics

Early childhood
education setting

Home and
community

8657part 1b.qxd 04/07/2006 18:12 Page 80

Free download pdf