Sketch Book for the Artist

(singke) #1

OBJECTS


AND


INSTRUMENTS


Light and Illusions


SEEING IS MIRACULOUS. Upside-down patterns of light on

our retinas, transmitted to the brain as chains of electrical

impulses, are translated into a world of space, atmosphere,

form, and movement. To interpret visual stimulus, our

brains search for nameable things, and are quick to

propose ideas. We will even see things that do not exist

because minimal information suggests they are likely to be

there. We only need small prompts; hence our recognition

of a cartoon face composed of three lines, and our ability

to see forms in clouds or fire.

The outermost edge of our vision, less developed than the

center, reads only movement and can often make mistakes.

Someone passes our open door, perhaps; we turn our head

but no one was there. The slightest recognition is instantly

matched to our wealth of experience and expectation, and

we can be fooled. Here we look briefly at our perception of

light and darkness and our interpretation of diagrammatic

illusions. Through these we can enjoy witnessing our own

deception and bafflement and understand certain factors

that are very useful in picture-making.

LIGHT AND SHADOW
We learn to read light and shadow in our environment as
indicators of solidity and depth of space. Artists emulate and
manipulate this effect in their pictures. Before starting a tonal
drawing of any subject, think about its illumination. Are you happy

with what you see? Does the light enhance the subject? Could
you alter and improve it? On pp.102-03 deep shadows cast across
shells make their form easier to perceive. Below, a lit photograph
of an egg shows essential points to observe in tonal drawing.

The egg's darkest region is seen
against a lighter background.
The two tones meet without
a division or outline between.

The bottom-left quarter of the egg
lightens slightly toward its "edge"
when seen against the darker
shadow cast on the paper. The rim
of light on the egg is reflected light
received from surrounding paper.

The egg's lightest region is seen
against a darker background. Run
your eye around the circumference
of the egg. See how its "edge"
smoothly changes from light
against dark to dark against light.

Below the bottom "edge" of the
egg there is a rim of light reflected
onto the paper by the underside
of the egg.

Gray squares
The perceived tone of a black white,
gray, or colored surface is never
constant. Three factors change it:
the amount of light it receives; the
adjustment of our eyes (in growing
accustomed to a level of light); and
the proximity of contrasting tones.
Here, two identical mid-gray squares
are seen against black and paler gray
squares. The small squares are
identical in tone, but do not appear
so. We perceive one to be lighter
than the other. This effect is caused
by the contrast of their surroundings.

Card experiment
To further examine changes in tone,
try this experiment. On a cloudy day,
choose a window through which you
can see the sky. Take two opaque
cards the same size, one black and one
white. Tape them to the glass. From
across the room both will appear
gray against the sky. Remove the
black card and fold and retape it with
half sticking out. Adjust the angle of the
protruding part and again step back. At
one point you will catch enough light
on the black card to make it appear
lighter than the white one.
Free download pdf