Saylor URL: http://www.saylor.org/books Saylor.org
[3] Bushnell, I. W. R., Sai, F., & Mullin, J. T. (1989). Neonatal recognition of the mother’s face. British Journal of developmental
psychology, 7, 3–15.
[4] Gibson, E. J., Rosenzweig, M. R., & Porter, L. W. (1988). Exploratory behavior in the development of perceiving, acting, and
the acquiring of knowledge. In Annual review of psychology (Vol. 39, pp. 1–41). Palo Alto, CA: Annual Reviews; Gibson, E. J., &
Pick, A. D. (2000). An ecological approach to perceptual learning and development. New York, NY: Oxford University Press;
Smith, L. B., & Thelen, E. (2003). Development as a dynamic system. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(8), 343–348.
[5] Juraska, J. M., Henderson, C., & Müller, J. (1984). Differential rearing experience, gender, and radial maze
performance. Developmental Psychobiology, 17(3), 209–215.
[6] Soska, K. C., Adolph, K. E., & Johnson, S. P. (2010). Systems in development: Motor skill acquisition facilitates three-
dimensional object completion. Developmental Psychology, 46(1), 129–138.
[7] James, W. (1890). The principles of psychology. New York, NY: Dover.
[8] Trehub, S., & Rabinovitch, M. (1972). Auditory-linguistic sensitivity in early infancy.Developmental Psychology, 6(1), 74–77.
[9] Wynn, K. (1995). Infants possess a system of numerical knowledge. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 4, 172–176.
[10] DeLoache, J. S. (1987). Rapid change in the symbolic functioning of very young children. Science, 238(4833), 1556–1556.
[11] Klahr, D., & McWhinney, B. (1998). Information Processing. In D. Kuhn & R. S. Siegler (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology:
Cognition, perception, & language (5th ed., Vol. 2, pp. 631–678). New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons; Shrager, J., & Siegler, R. S.
(1998). SCADS: A model of children’s strategy choices and strategy discoveries. Psychological Science, 9, 405–422.
[12] Driscoll, M. P. (1994). Psychology of learning for instruction. Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon; Levin, I., Siegler, S. R., & Druyan, S.
(1990). Misconceptions on motion: Development and training effects. Child Development, 61, 1544–1556.
[13] Baillargeon, R. (2004). Infants’ physical world. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 13(3), 89–94; Wang, S. H.,
Baillargeon, R., & Brueckner, L. (2004). Young infants’ reasoning about hidden objects: Evidence from violation-of-expectation
tasks with test trials only. Cognition, 93, 167–198.
[14] Courage, M. L., & Howe, M. L. (2002). From infant to child: The dynamics of cognitive change in the second year of
life. Psychological Bulletin, 128(2), 250–276.
[15] Dasen, P. R. (1972). Cross-cultural Piagetian research: A summary. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 3, 23–39.
[16] Cole, M. (1996). Culture in mind. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press; Rogoff, B. (1990). Apprenticeship in thinking:
Cognitive development in social context. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; Tomasello, M. (1999). The cultural origins of
human cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.