Saylor URL: http://www.saylor.org/books Saylor.org
Cognitive process Description Potential threat to accuracy
Misinformation
effect
Errors in memory that occur when new but
incorrect information influences existing
accurate memories
Eyewitnesses who are questioned by the police
may change their memories of what they
observed at the crime scene.
Overconfidence
When we are more certain that our memories
and judgments are accurate than we should be
Eyewitnesses may be very confident that they
have accurately identified a suspect, even
though their memories are incorrect.
Salience
When some stimuli, (e.g., those that are colorful,
moving, or unexpected) grab our attention and
make them more likely to be remembered
We may base our judgments on a single salient
event while we ignore hundreds of other equally
informative events that we do not see.
Representativeness
heuristic
Tendency to make judgments according to how
well the event matches our expectations
After a coin has come up “heads” many times in
a row, we may erroneously think that the next
flip is more likely to be “tails” (the gambler’s
fallacy).
Availability heuristic
Idea that things that come to mind easily are
seen as more common
We may overestimate the crime statistics in our
own area, because these crimes are so easy to
recall.
Cognitive
accessibility
Idea that some memories are more highly
activated than others
We may think that we contributed more to a
project than we really did because it is so easy to
remember our own contributions.
Counterfactual
thinking
When we “replay” events such that they turn out
differently (especially when only minor changes
in the events leading up to them make a
difference)
We may feel particularly bad about events that
might not have occurred if only a small change
had occurred before them.
Source Monitoring: Did It Really Happen?
One potential error in memory involves mistakes in differentiating the sources of
information. Source monitoring refers to the ability to accurately identify the source of a
memory. Perhaps you’ve had the experience of wondering whether you really experienced an
event or only dreamed or imagined it. If so, you wouldn’t be alone. Rassin, Merkelbach, and
Spaan (2001) [1] reported that up to 25% of college students reported being confused about real
versus dreamed events. Studies suggest that people who are fantasy-prone are more likely to
experience source monitoring errors (Winograd, Peluso, & Glover, 1998), [2] and such errors also