Science - USA (2021-10-29)

(Antfer) #1
INSIGHTS | PERSPECTIVES

science.org SCIENCE

By Jiyun N. Shin1,2, Guy Doron3,4,5,
Matthew E. Larkum4,5

M


emories are generally considered
to be embodied in the connection
strengths between neurons. Finding
the exact synaptic connections re-
sponsible for memories out of hun-
dreds of trillions at first appears
impossible. But new research may be taking
us a step closer. Specific brain areas under
the neocortex are known to be required for
memory formation (including the hippo-
campus and basal ganglia), while long-term
memory relating to facts and knowledge (se-
mantic memory) is thought to reside in the
neocortex ( 1 ). However, the precise location
of semantic memory is still unknown, pre-
venting researchers from understanding how
and where memories are formed.
Accumulating evidence in rodents sug-
gests that neocortical layer 1, the outermost
layer of the brain, might be a key site for
long-term plasticity ( 2 ) and learning ( 3 , 4 ).
Common to all these studies is the observa-
tion that subcortical memory structures pre-
dominantly target this layer. For instance,
higher-order thalamic input targets layer 1
in the sensory cortex, where it has also been
associated with learning ( 5 ) and enhanced
activity in the tuft dendrites of pyramidal
neurons, the principal excitatory neurons of
the neocortex ( 2 ). Similarly, it was shown that
projections from the amygdala to layer 1 are
crucial for fear memory formation ( 6 ). More
recently, it was shown that the hippocampus
also targets neocortical layer 1 via parahippo-
campal structures and is crucial for associa-
tive learning ( 7 ). This leads to the hypothesis
that layer 1 is the locus of semantic memory
formation and storage in the neocortex,
where the synapses encoding memories are
likely to be found.
Layer 1 of the neocortex is enigmatic, and
there is still no consensus about its function.
It stands out as the only layer almost devoid
of cell bodies, which makes it even more
surprising that it is the target of so many

long-range neuronal axon fibers. These fibers
tend to come from higher cortical areas and
higher-order thalamic nuclei and are known
to convey context-related feedback informa-
tion ( 8 ) and correlate with learning ( 5 , 9 , 10 ).
Layer 1 consists of a dense complex of over-
laying dendrites made up of the tops of pyra-
midal neurons (tuft dendrites). Thus, it is this
part of the pyramidal neurons that receives
feedback information, whereas the bottom
of these neurons primarily receives feature-
related, feed-forward information. Therefore,
the pyramidal neuron is ideally positioned to
link the two information streams ( 8 ).
The question remains: Why would memory-
related structures target context in layer 1
rather than features encoded in the lower
layers? Adding memory to an existing cogni-
tive theory describing the role of cortical den-
drites in perceptual experience may provide
the answer ( 11 ). This theory of perception
suggests a functional definition of “feature”
and associated “context,” where a feature is
defined as the primary output of a cortical
module (“column”), and context is defined as
relevant information from across the brain
that arrives in layer 1 of that column. Thus,
for an “orange column” responding during
a perceptual experience, “shape” would be a
part of the context for this column. This per-
ceptual theory explains how the properties of
pyramidal neurons bind context to features.
Linking this theory to memory could ex-
plain why and how memory structures in-
fluence layer 1 of the neocortex. There are
two possible scenarios: the memory content
scenario, in which memory structures pro-
jecting to layer 1 directly provide additional
context; or the memory switch scenario, in
which memory structures modulate or gate
other context-related inputs to layer 1. In
both scenarios, experiences would involve
the short-term association of context with
features, and memory would involve encod-
ing and stabilization of this association. For
example, recognizing a tiger should evoke
distributed neuronal activity around the cor-
tex according to the features accessible via
the senses and the context derived from pre-
viously learned associations (see the figure).
Seeing a tiger for the first time would involve
temporary associations that would need to be
formed or stabilized.
This implies that associative memory is the
convergence of different types of information
from various cortical and subcortical sources

to layer 1 and is therefore intrinsically dis-
tributed, even in a given cortical area. It is
possible that the different memory-related
brain structures projecting to layer 1 provide
different criteria for stabilization of context
association (novelty detection, importance,
emotional valence, or deviation from expec-
tation, etc.). Overall, semantic memory is hy-
pothesized to be the long-term association of
different contexts with particular features in
neocortical layer 1.
The two scenarios entail certain predic-
tions. The memory content scenario pre-
dicts that layer 1 inputs constitute memory
content themselves, show long-term plastic-
ity, and require the source structure to be
activated during retrieval. Evidence for the
memory content scenario has been observed
directly for thalamic projections to layer 1
( 5 ). Conversely, the memory switch scenario
explains how memory retrieval could occur
without the source structure. A recent study
showed that learning to associate a stimulus
with a reward requires hippocampal input
to layer 1 via the medial temporal lobe struc-
tures but could subsequently be retrieved
without this input ( 7 ).
It is possible that the two scenarios could
operate in parallel. For example, it is well
established that the hippocampus mediates
spatiotemporal context that is an important
aspect of episodic autobiographical memory
( 1 ). Thus, inputs to layer 1 could provide both
a criterion for consolidating associated con-
text (memory switch scenario) and a con-
stitutive part of the context itself (memory
content scenario), providing the autobio-
graphical component by linking time and lo-
cation to memories.
The precise mechanisms of memory
stabilization in layer 1 remain unknown.
Stabilization may involve the up- or down-
regulation of synaptic inputs to the densely
packed tuft dendrites of pyramidal neurons
in layer 1 ( 9 ), or possibly more-complex cir-
cuit refinements involving heterosynaptic
plasticity induction including other cortical
layers ( 2 ). There is also evidence that local
inhibition mechanisms in layer 1 can shape
and modulate long-range contextual inputs
( 5 ). Layer 1 inhibitory neurons are optimally
positioned to determine synaptic plasticity
via calcium-dependent signaling in the py-
ramidal cell tuft dendrites. Indeed, several
classes of inhibitory neurons reside in or in-
fluence layer 1 ( 8 ). These inhibitory sources

(^1) New York University Comprehensive Epilepsy Center,
New York, NY, USA.^2 Department of Neurology, New York
University School of Medicine, New York, NY, USA.^3 Scientific
and Competitive Intelligence, R&D, Pharmaceuticals, Bayer
AG, Berlin, Germany.^4 Institute for Biology, Humboldt
University of Berlin, Berlin, Germany.^5 NeuroCure Cluster
of Excellence, Charité – Universitätsmedizin Berlin, Berlin,
Germany. Email: [email protected];
[email protected]
NEUROSCIENCE
Memories off the top of your head
Cortical layer 1 has a special role in long-term memory
538 29 OCTOBER 2021 • VOL 374 ISSUE 6567

Free download pdf