content words but featuring three different
syntactic structures: a coordinated clause (e.g.,
“The writer admires the poet and writes the
paper”), a subject-relative clause (e.g.,“The
writer that admires the poet writes the paper”),
or an object-relative clause (e.g.,“The writer
that the poet admires writes the paper”; Table
1A). Each sentence was followed by a test af-
firmation (e.g.,“The poet admires the writer”;
Table 1B), which the participants had to judge
as true or false with respect to the immediately
preceding sentence. The participants succeeded
Thibaultet al.,Science 374 , eabe0874 (2021) 12 November 2021 2 of 14
A B
C
D E
F
GHI
Fig. 1. Experiment 1: Overlap of syntax and tool-use planning activity.(A) Setup
and experimental design of the syntactic task. (B)d′(left graph) and RTs (right
graph) for the syntactic comprehension of the three sentence structures. Both the
one-way rmANOVA ond′and the LMM on RTs showed a significant main effect of
Sentence(d′:F(1.6,30.6)= 40.04;P< 0.001;ƞG^2 = 0.49; RTs:c^2 (2)= 25.21,P< 0.001).
Error bars show SEs. ***P< 0.001, Tukey’s post hoc test. (C) Statistical maps
thresholded atP< 0.001, uncorrected, for syntax (object relatives compared with the
two other sentence types). (D) Setup and experimental design of tool-use and free-
hand motor tasks. (E) Statistical maps thresholded atP< 0.001, uncorrected,
for tool-use planning. (F) Joint neuronal activity for syntax and tool-use planning in
the BG, thresholded atP< 0.001, uncorrected. (GtoI) Average brain activity
level in each cluster significantly activated by both tasks. The highest activations
were found for tool-use planning (blue) and object-relative clauses (yellow) in
the motor and syntactic tasks, respectively.
RESEARCH | RESEARCH ARTICLE