Abnormal Psychology

(やまだぃちぅ) #1

Anxiety Disorders 291


rate and blood pressure, which often causes fainting. Among people with this type of


phobia, over half report having fainted in response to a feared stimulus (Öst, 1992).


Situational Type


Asituational type of specifi c phobia involves a fear of a particular situation, such as be-


ing in an airplane, elevator, or enclosed space, or of driving a car. Some people develop


this type of phobia in childhood, but in general it has a later onset, often in the mid-20s.


People with this type tend to experience more panic attacks than do people with other


types of specifi c phobia (Lipsitz et al., 2002). Situational phobia has a gender ratio, age


of onset, and family history similar to those of panic disorder with agoraphobia.


Some researchers have argued that situational and natural environment fears

overlap; for instance, fear of the dark is both a situational and a natural environ-


ment phobia. These researchers propose that these two types should be combined


or that there should be no types, and mental health professionals should simply


name the feared stimuli specifi cally (Antony & Swinson, 2000b).


Other Type


This category includes any other type of specifi c phobia that does not fall into the


four categories already discussed. Examples of specifi c phobias that would be clas-


sifi ed as “other” are a fear of falling down when not near a wall or some other type


of support, a fear of costumed characters (such as clowns at a circus), and a pho-


bic avoidance of situations that may lead to choking, vomiting, or contracting an


illness—the last of which Hughes may have had.


Specifi c Phobias


As noted in Table 7.11, the majority of people who have one sort of specifi c phobia are


likely to have at least one more (Stinson et al., 2007). This high comorbidity among


specifi c phobias has led some researchers to suggest that, like social phobia, specifi c


phobia may take two forms: a focused type that is limited to a specifi c stimulus, and a


more generalized type that involves fear of various stimuli (Stinson et al., 2007).


The unrealistic fears and extreme anxiety of a specifi c phobia occur in the pres-

ence of the feared stimulus but may even occur when simply thinking about it. Often,


people with a specifi c phobia fear that something bad will happen as a result of contact


with the stimulus: “What if I get stuck in the tunnel and it cracks open and fl oods?”


“What if the spider bites me and I get a deadly disease?” People may also be afraid


of the consequences of their reaction to the phobic stimulus, such as losing control of


themselves or not being able to get help: “What if I mess my pants after the spider bites


me?” or “What if I faint or have a heart attack while I’m in the tunnel?” In this sense,


the fear of somehow losing control is similar to that in panic disorder (Horwath et al.,


1993). The situation or object that causes fear and anxiety is related to the content of


the worry about losing control: Someone who is afraid of heights may worry about


getting dizzy when high up (and, as a result, plummeting to the ground).


There is a very long list of stimuli to which people have developed phobias (see

http://www.phobialist.com),,) but people do not seem to develop specifi c phobias toward


all kinds of stimuli. Humans, like other animals, have a natural readiness for cer-


tain stimuli to produce certain conditioned responses. This preparedness means that


less learning from experience is needed to produce the conditioning (Öhman et al.,


1976). Young children, for example, typically go through a period when they are


easily afraid of the dark or of storms, which may suggest that such fears can more


readily become specifi c phobias. In contrast, a fear of fl owers is extremely unusual.


Some psychologists (Menzies & Parker, 2001; Öhman, 1986; Stein & Matsunaga,



  1. propose that such preparedness has an evolutionary advantage—people are


more readily afraid of objects or situations that could lead to death, such as being too


close to the edge of a cliff (and falling off) or being bitten by a poisonous snake or spi-


der. According to this view, those among our early ancestors who were afraid of these


stimuli and avoided them were more likely to survive and reproduce—and thus pass


on genes that led them to be prepared to fear these stimuli.


Table 7.11 • Specifi c Phobia
Facts at a Glance

Prevalence


  • Approximately 10% of Americans will
    experience in their lifetime a fear severe
    enough to meet the criteria for specifi c
    phobia (Stinson et al., 2007).


Comorbidity


  • Only a quarter of those with a diagnosis
    of specifi c phobia have a single specifi c
    phobia; 50% have three or more pho-
    bias. In addition, the more phobias a
    person has, the more likely he or she is
    to have another type of anxiety disorder
    (Curtis et al., 1998; Stinson et al., 2007).


Onset


  • There are different ages of onset for
    the various types of specifi c phobias,
    although the average age is about 10
    years (Stinson et al., 2007).

  • Specifi c phobias that arise after trauma
    can occur at any age.


Course


  • Specifi c phobias that arise during ado-
    lescence are likely to persist through
    adulthood; only 20% of persistent
    phobias that begin in adolescence will
    improve without treatment.


Gender Differences


  • Twice as many women are diagnosed
    with specifi c phobias as men, although
    this ratio varies across type of specifi c
    phobias (Stinson et al., 2007). The
    gender difference in prevalence rates is
    more pronounced with animal, natural
    environment, and situational phobias.
    Men and women are equally likely to
    report blood-injection-injury phobia
    (Fredrikson et al., 1996).

  • Gender differences may refl ect a report-
    ing bias: Women may be more likely to
    report symptoms, but not necessarily
    more likely to have them (Hartung &
    Widiger, 1998).


Cultural Differences


  • The prevalence rates of the various types
    of specifi c phobias vary across countries,
    suggesting that cultural factors, such as
    the likelihood of coming into contact with
    various stimuli, affect the form that spe-
    cifi c phobias take (Chambers, Yeragani, &
    Keshavan, 1986).
    Source: Unless otherwise noted, information in the
    table is from American Psychiatric Association, 2000.

Free download pdf