Abnormal Psychology

(やまだぃちぅ) #1

Childhood Disorders 645


Brain Systems


In most forms of dyslexia, the brain systems involved in auditory processing do not


function as they should (Marshall et al., 2008; Ramus et al., 2003). For example,


one study used electrodes placed on the scalp to examine the brain waves of infants


while they listened to syllables coming out of a speaker. Eight years later, the chil-


dren’s reading abilities were assessed, and the children classifi ed as dyslexic, poor


readers, or normal readers. The children who were classifi ed as dyslexic at 8 years


old had brain-wave patterns in infancy (while they listened to spoken syllables) that


were different from those of the children whose reading ability was classifi ed as


normal, which suggests that the children with dyslexia were born with processing


problems in the auditory system (Molfese, 2000). Further research has suggested


that these brain-wave differences continue at least through the fi rst 4 years of life


(Espy et al. 2004).


The results of many neuroimaging studies have converged to identify a set of brain

areas that is disrupted in people who have dyslexia (Shaywitz, Lyon, & Shaywitz,


2006). First, two rear areas in the left hemisphere are not as strongly activated during


reading tasks in people with dyslexia as they are in people who read normally. One of


these areas, at the junction of the parietal and temporal lobes, appears to be involved


in converting visual input to sounds (Friedman, Ween, & Albert, 1993). The other


area, at the junction of the parietal and occipital lobes, appears to be used to recognize


whole words, based on their visual forms (Cao et al., 2006; McCandliss, Cohen, &


Dehaene, 2003). Moreover, these areas are not activated normally even in young chil-


dren with dyslexia, and thus the malfunction observed in adults cannot be a result


of not reading properly over the course of many years but probably contributes to


reading disorder (Shaywitz et al., 2002). Second, two other brain areas (the bottom


part of the frontal lobe and the right occipital-temporal region) are more activated in


people with a reading disorder than in people who read normally. These areas appear


to be used in carrying out compensatory strategies, which rely on stored information


instead of the usual vision-sound conversion process.


Consistent with the neuroimaging results, researchers have also reported struc-

tural differences between the brains of people with dyslexia and normal readers.


Compared to people who read normally, people with dyslexia have reduced gray


matter (which includes the cell bodies of neurons) in the temporal lobes, particu-


larly the left temporal lobe (Vinckenbosch, Robichon, & Eliez, 2005), and portions


of their frontal lobes are relatively large (Vinckenbosch, Robichon, & Eliez, 2005;


Zadina et al., 2006). Moreover, people who have relatively large occipital lobes


(which are specialized for vision) tend to read better than those with smaller occipi-


tal lobes (Zadina et al., 2006); this difference may suggest that at least some people


with dyslexia have impaired visual abilities. (Fine et al., 2007).


However, the precise brain areas involved in dyslexia are infl uenced by culture,

as manifested by the language spoken in a society. Specifi cally, part of the left fron-


tal lobe is impaired in dyslexic children who speak Chinese, instead of the areas


just discussed (which were assessed in English-speaking children). Chinese writing


does not depend on an alphabet, but instead requires memorizing specifi c characters


that correspond to words (Siok et al., 2004). The frontal lobes are involved in using


stored information to help register current stimuli, and such processing may play a


large role in reading Chinese characters.


At least some forms of dyslexia also appear to reflect a specific problem in

processing visual stimuli—independent of problems in connecting those stimuli to


sounds—which may be why some people with dyslexia reverse letters when they


write (Vidyasagar, 2005). Thus, fi ndings regarding brain systems suggest that there


are probably different forms of dyslexia, with different underlying causes.


Genetics


Reading disorder, and possibly the other learning disorders, are moderately to


highly heritable (Hawke, Wadsworth, & DeFries, 2006; Schulte-Körne, 2001), and


at least four specifi c genes are thought to affect the development of these disorders


P S

N
Free download pdf