Childhood Disorders 661
disorder have smaller brains than do children and adolescents without the disorder,
and the defi cit in size is particularly marked in the frontal lobes (Schneider, Retz
et al., 2006; Sowell et al., 2003; Valera et al., 2007). Indeed, particular parts of
the frontal lobes have been shown to be smaller in adults with ADHD (Durston et
al., 2004; Hesslinger et al., 2002). However, the anatomical abnormalities are not
restricted to the frontal lobes. Parts of the temporal lobes are smaller than normal
in children and adolescents who have the disorder (Sowell et al., 2003), as are por-
tions of the corpus callosum, the basal ganglia, and the cerebellum (Castellanos et
al., 2002; Schrimsher et al., 2002; Valera et al., 2007). The cerebellar defi cit is par-
ticularly interesting because this brain structure is crucial to attention and timing; in
fact, the smaller this structure, the worse the symptoms of ADHD are (Castellanos
et al., 2002; Mackie et al., 2007).
Some of the brain differences between people who have ADHD and those
who do not may actually arise because the brain has attempted to compensate for
the impaired regions. Perhaps critically, at least some of the differences found in the
brains of children—notably those in the basal ganglia—do not persist into adult-
hood (Castellanos et al., 2002; Schneider et al., 2006). This fi nding is consistent
with the fact that over half of the children who are diagnosed with ADHD do not
meet the diagnostic criteria when they become adults (Schneider et al., 2006).
Regarding brain function, the research results indicate that ADHD is not a result
of impaired functioning in any single brain area, but rather emerges from how different
areas interact. As the anatomical abnormalities would suggest, neuroimaging studies
have revealed many patterns of abnormal brain functioning in people who have ADHD
(Rubia et al., 2007; Stevens, Pearlson, & Kiehl, 2007; Vance et al., 2007). In general,
neural structures involved in attention, including portions of the frontal and parietal
lobes, tend not to be activated as strongly (during relevant tasks) in people with this
disorder as in people without it (e.g., Stevens, Pearlson, & Kiehl, 2007; Schneider et al.,
2006; Vance et al., 2007). However, virtually every lobe in the brains of individuals
with ADHD has been shown not to function normally during tasks that draw on their
functions (e.g., Mulas et al., 2006; Schneider et al., 2006; Vance et al., 2007).
In addition, abnormal brain functioning can infl uence the autonomic nervous
system (see Chapter 2): ADHD (and some types of conduct disorder) has been as-
sociated with unusually low arousal in response to normal levels of stimulation
(Crowell et al., 2006), a response that could explain some of the stimulation-seeking
behavior seen in individuals with this disorder. That is, these people could engage in
stimulation-seeking behavior in order to obtain an optimal level of arousal.
ADHD and Neural Communication
The overall pattern of diffi culties that characterizes ADHD suggests problems with
multiple neurotransmitters that are involved in coordinating and organizing cogni-
tion and behavior. For one, dopamine apparently does not function effectively in
the brains of people with this disorder (Volkow et al., 2007). This malfunction may
arise for any of various reasons, including too few of the relevant receptors or prob-
lems in removing dopamine from the synapse (Swanson et al., 2007). But dopamine
functioning is not the only issue: Imbalances in serotonin and norepinephrine may
also contribute to the disorder (Arnsten, 2006; Gainetdinov et al., 1999; Waldman
& Gizer, 2006), and the activity of other neurotransmitters may also be disrupted
(Pattij & Vanderschuren, 2008). Given the number of brain areas that are involved,
it is not surprising that problems with multiple neurotransmitters are likely to be
associated with the disorder.
ADHD and Genetics
Genes may be one reason why people with ADHD have abnormal brain systems and
disrupted neural communication. Indeed, not only does this disorder runs in families,
but also parent and teacher reports indicate that it is highly correlated among mo-
nozygotic twins (with correlations ranging from 0.60 to 0.90). In addition, a large
set of data reveals that this disorder is among the most heritable of psychological dis-
orders (Martin et al., 2006; Stevenson et al., 2005; Waldman & Gizer, 2006).
P S
N