THINKING THROUGH DRAWING: PRACTICE INTO KNOWLEDGE

(Jeff_L) #1

68 TEACHERs COLLEGE COLUmbIA UNIvERsITy


Learning to Pause


ist both (A) learns, and works within, the limits of
their perception, and (B) extends these limits by
developing their perception. Central to the argu-
ment is an enactive account of vision, entailing that
the way the body moves creates changes in percep-
tual awareness. From this view the role of the hand
is always a significant part of the perceptual equa-
tion. Only by considering the dance of eye, hand,
head and whole body can a complete picture begin
to emerge of the drawing process, of perceptual
style and transformations.


Extra-ordinary perception
Van Sommers (1984) argues, in the particu-
lar context of drawing a hand, “I do not believe
that normal perceptual commerce with objects is
adequate to this task” (132) and that while several
styles of perceptual analysis “would be adequate for
recognition... not all are equally suitable as a basis
for drawing” (132).
What is an adequate and suitable style of per-
ception for accurate drawing from life, and how
do we acquire this style? My original proposal
was to study how experienced drawers look at gaps
between objects, referred to by drawing teachers as
“negative space.”
The dominant paradigm in drawing teaching is
that students need to learn how to look at things
in order to draw them. The premise is that if you


learn to look then you can draw, implying that the
mechanical act of the hand will follow easily. There
is a bias towards isolating the eye as the sole per-
ceptual tool in the task of drawing. The common
view is that the eye perceives, and the hand fol-
lows. To a large extent experimental research has
operated on this paradigm, adopting a sequential
model with the eye looking and perceiving, and the
action of the hand following information from the
eye. My perspective, from my own teaching experi-
ence and research, is that students often struggle to
integrate and coordinate their eyes and hands, and
that being more explicit about how the hand and
eye synchronize will facilitate learning. Cognitive
scientists have begun a productive dialogue with
philosophers about skill acquisition, embodiment,
enactive vision and consciousness (see Jacob and
Jeannerod, 2003; Seeley and Kozbelt, 2008; Varela
et al., 1993; O’Regan and Noë, 2001) however this
does not, I submit, reach drawing education in any
effective way.
The first point to bear in mind is that the eye
moves a great deal during observational drawing,
making many fixations and weaving a web of con-
nections.
The movements of drawing (a style) bring about
a new style of perception. Crucially this includes
eye movements, and phases of not moving.

Segmenting and pausing distinguishes
experts from novices
Two behavioural factors distinguish experts
from novices: pausing more often, and drawing
smaller segments of lines. Tchalenko (2009) found
differences in eye movements between novice and
expert drawers when copying complex lines, and
developed a drawing hypothesis relating to how
people divide up the image into sections to be
drawn. He recorded the drawing strategies of 16
subjects, with drawing experience ranging from
novice to expert, while they copied a line drawing
of a standing woman. He found significant differ-
ences between the experts and novices:

The experts produced accurate cop-
ies whereas all the beginners produced
marked inaccuracies of overall scaling,
proportion and shape. Analysis of eye and
hand movements showed that the experts
alone segmented the original drawing into
simple line sections that were copied one

Figure 2. Still of eye tracking data imposed on video
footage, showing the web the eye weaves between
object and drawing (Brew & Fava, 2011)
Free download pdf