The Psychology of Gender 4th Edition

(Tuis.) #1
Achievement 213

noted whether the feedback was positive or
negative and whether it pertained to the chil-
dren’s intellectual performance or to nonin-
tellectual aspects of performance. Feedback
about nonintellectual aspects often pertained
to conduct, as in “Johnny, please settle down
and sit in your chair,” or appearance: “Mary,
you have a lovely outfit on today.” The inves-
tigators found no difference in the amount
of positive or negative feedback given to
boys and girls, but an important difference
in whether the feedback pertained to intel-
lectual or nonintellectual aspects of the chil-
dren’s performance. For girls, only 30% of
the negative feedback pertained to nonintel-
lectual aspects of performance, whereas 70%
pertained to intellectual aspects of perfor-
mance. For boys, 67% of the negative feed-
back pertained to nonintellectual aspects of
performance, whereas only 33% pertained
to intellectual aspects of performance. The
authors suggested these differences make
negative feedback a very salient indicator of
poor performance for girls but an unclear in-
dicator of poor performance for boys. When
girls receive negative feedback, it is more
likely to be related to their schoolwork than
work-irrelevant domains, such as conduct
or appearance; thus girls take negative feed-
back seriously. Boys, by contrast, are able to
discount negative feedback because it usually
has nothing to do with the intellectual as-
pects of their performance. Thus, when boys
receive negative feedback about their work,
they can reason, “The teacher doesn’t like
me. She is always criticizing me. She tells me
to dress neater and to be quieter. What does
she know about whether or not I can read?”
In the same study, positive feedback typi-
cally pertained to intellectual aspects of per-
formance for both boys and girls. However,
when compared to the positive feedback boys
received, proportionally more of girls’ positive
feedback concerned nonintellectual aspects of

end up affecting how girls and boys respond
to the feedback they receive from teachers
about their academics. This was shown in
an early observational study that has now
become a classic in the field (Dweck et al.,
1978). Two raters observed instances of
evaluative feedback given to children and

DO GENDER 6.4

Classroom Behavior

Conduct your own observational study of
classroom behavior. Record some or all of
the following, noting whether the inter-
action involved a female or male student.
Are there other features of teacher-student
interactions worth observing?


  1. Teacher calling on a student.

  2. Teacher giving praise to a student.

  3. Teacher criticizing a student.

  4. Length of time the teacher waits for
    a response after calling on a student.

  5. Nature of the teacher’s response to a
    student’s response (praises, criticizes,
    expands on, ignores the response).

  6. Number of times the teacher
    interrupts a student.

  7. Number of times the student
    interrupts the teacher or another
    student.

  8. Student raising a hand.

  9. Student shouting out an answer.
    After conducting your observational
    study, you might also administer a ques-
    tionnaire to the teacher and the students
    asking whether they observed different
    frequencies of behavior with male and
    female students. You can then compare
    your observational data to the student and
    teacher self-report data.


M06_HELG0185_04_SE_C06.indd 213 6/21/11 8:10 AM

Free download pdf