Encyclopedia of Psychology and Law

(lily) #1
but at the same time, these drawings may also increase
inaccuracies in children’s testimony.
In summary, research has shown that props and draw-
ings can, under certain circumstances, facilitate memory
accuracy in children older than 5 years, whereas they may
add error to the reports of younger children. Although
there is currently no “gold standard” method of interview-
ing children, different combinations of free-recall, spe-
cific, and prop-assisted questions are being researched to
determine which of them facilitates the most accurate and
complete memory reports from children.

PPhhoottoo LLiinneeuuppss
When interviewed in forensic situations, children
may be presented with photographic lineups to identify
culprits. A lineup may include a criminal (target-
present lineup) or only innocent individuals (target-
absent lineup). When they are shown a target-present
lineup, preschool-age children are less likely than
adults to make correct identifications, although children
around age 5 and above are typically comparable with
adults in making correct identifications. Shown a
target-absent lineup, however, even early adolescents
are inferior to adults, making fewer correct rejections
and more false identifications. As with leading ques-
tions, the photo lineup may entice children to guess.
Witnesses are usually shown a simultaneous lineup,
in which all lineup members are presented at once and
only one decision is made. This method has been crit-
icized for encouraging a relative judgment, whereby
witnesses compare all lineup members and choose the
member who looks most like the criminal relative to
other members. Although this strategy is successful in
target-present lineups, it may lead to errors in target-
absent lineups. Fortunately, fairly simple training
techniques can reduce guessing on target-absent line-
ups in older children.
An alternative procedure is the sequential lineup, in
which witnesses are shown photographs one at a time
and make a decision for each photograph. Compared
with simultaneous lineups, sequential lineups reduce
adults’ false identifications by increasing correct
rejections of target-absent lineups while having a min-
imal effect on correct identifications from target-
present lineups. When they are shown a sequential
lineup, witnesses might make an absolute judgment
for each photograph by comparing the photograph
with their recollection of the criminal. However, target-
absent errors by children are not reduced in sequential
compared with simultaneous procedures.

Children in the Courtroom

As a result of involvement with legal authorities,
children may experience social and emotional distress.
Although repeated interviewing of children can keep
accurate memories alive, child victims report that
being interviewed multiple times by legal authorities is
stressful for them. Speaking about traumatic experi-
ences (particularly in open court), lack of parental sup-
port, harsh cross-examination, facing the defendant,
and not being believed add to children’s distress and
may reduce significantly the amount of information
provided by child witnesses. Moreover, in child sexual
abuse cases, a child’s initial disclosure of the abuse to
a parent, teacher, or other trusted adult may include a
more detailed account than the testimony that the child
gives in court months or even years later. Although tes-
tifying may be helpful for some children, it causes oth-
ers to recover from the criminal and legal experience
more slowly than their nontestifying counterparts.
Child sexual abuse victims who had to testify multiple
times in severe intrafamilial cases tend to have the
most negative long-term emotional effects and are thus
most in need of protection during criminal prosecu-
tions. To remedy these negative consequences, proce-
dural modifications (e.g., testifying via closed-circuit
television) and multidisciplinary investigations, con-
ducted at child advocacy centers and involving teams
of legal professionals (e.g., the police, prosecuting
attorneys, and child protective services workers), who
coordinate their efforts into a single interview of the
child victim/witness, are being developed and tested in
the United States and abroad.
Having an adult (e.g., a mother, social worker, or
police officer) recount children’s out-of-court state-
ments (e.g., hearsay) at trial has recently attracted
research and legal interest. In criminal trials regarding
child sexual abuse, hearsay is often introduced in addi-
tion to the child’s live testimony. Although hearsay is
normally discouraged in the American legal system,
there are special hearsay exceptions, some of which
apply specifically to children’s statements. However, a
recent U.S. Supreme Court ruling suggests that if the
out-of-court statement was made to an authority (e.g.,
a forensic interviewer) and is thus “testimonial,” the
authority cannot testify in place of the child.
Mock jurors find children’s statements more credi-
ble when the child testifies live in court than if the
child is replaced by a hearsay witness. Mock jurors
also find children less credible if they testify via
closed-circuit television instead of face-to-face at

Children’s Testimony——— 81

C-Cutler (Encyc)-45463.qxd 11/18/2007 12:41 PM Page 81

Free download pdf