Encyclopedia of Psychology and Law

(lily) #1
processes (or approaches) that might answer that
question. According to the emotional approach(some-
times called the affective approach), the three most
common types of emotion associated with deception
are guilt, fear, and excitement. A liar might feel guilty
because he or she is lying, might be afraid of getting
caught, or might be excited about the possibility of
fooling someone (“duping delight”).
According to the cognitive complexity approach
(sometimes called cognitive load or working memory
model), the lie should be possible to detect from the
liars’ nonverbal behavior because it is more difficult to
lie than to tell the truth. The liars have to come up with
believable answers, avoid contradicting themselves,
and tell a lie consistent with what the interviewer
knows or might find out. Additionally, they have to
remember what they have said, in order to declare the
same things again if asked to repeat their statement.
The attempted control approach emphasizes that
liars may be concerned that their lies will be detected
by, for example, nonverbal behaviors and will there-
fore try to suppress such behaviors. In other words,
they will try to make a convincing impression by, for
example, suppressing their nervousness and masking
evidence of thinking hard. However, when controlling
their body language, liars may overcontrol their behav-
ior, therefore exhibiting body language that will appear
planned, rehearsed, and lacking in spontaneity. For
example, liars may believe that bodily movements will
give their lies away and will consequently avoid any
movements not strictly essential, resulting in rigidity.
All three processes may occur simultaneously.
That is, liars could—at the same time—be nervous,
have to think hard, and try to control themselves.
Which of these processes is most prevalent depends
on the situation. In high-stake lies, nervous responses
are more likely to occur. In complicated lies, indica-
tors of increased cognitive load are more likely to
occur. Attempts to control behavior, voice, and speech
may especially occur in motivated liars.
Before turning to the outcomes of reviews about
nonverbal behavior, it should be emphasized that the
approaches only suggest that the presence of signs of
emotions, content complexity, and impression manage-
ment may be indicative of deceit. None of these
approaches claim that the occurrence of these signs
necessarily indicates deception. Truth tellers might
experience exactly the same processes. For example,
innocent (truthful) suspects might very well be anxious
if they worry about not being believed in a police inter-
view. Because of that fear, they may show the same

nervous reactions as liars who are afraid of being
caught. The lie catcher is then put in a difficult position:
Should the nonverbal behaviors be interpreted as signs
of guilt or of innocence? The behavior does not provide
the answer. The false accusation of a truth teller on the
basis of the emotional reactions displayed has been
labeled the Othello error, after Shakespeare’s play.

Nonverbal Behavior
and Deception in General
The most notable result of research to date is that non-
verbal behaviors generally do not correlate strongly
with either deception or truthfulness; very few reliable
nonverbal cues to deception have been found.
There is evidence that liars tend to speak in a higher-
pitched voice,which might be the result of experienced
arousal. However, differences in pitch between liars
and truth tellers are usually small and detectable only
with technical equipment. Furthermore, sometimes
liars’voices sound tenser than truth tellers’ voices,
another result of arousal. Speech errors(e.g., word
and/or sentence repetition, sentence incompletion, slips
of the tongue) occur more often during deception, and
response latency is longer before giving deceptive
answers. There is also evidence of message duration
being shorter for liars, who also tend to make fewer
illustrators(hand and arm movements modifying what
is said verbally). The decrease in movements might be
the result of lie complexity or overcontrol of behavior.
Moreover, compared with truth tellers, liars tend to
sound vocally less expressive, more passive, and more
uncertain.This might all be the result of overcontrol of
behavior. Liars also sound less involved and come
across as being less cooperativeand tending to make
more negative statements.This might be caused by a
negative emotion felt by the liar.
Perhaps the most remarkable outcome of the liter-
ature reviews is the finding that several signs of ner-
vousness, such as gaze aversion (avoidance of eye
contact) and fidgeting, are generally unrelated to
deception. One reason why nervous behaviors do not
seem to be systematically related to deception is that
truth tellers could be nervous as well. Another reason
could be that in most deception studies, people are
requested to lie or tell the truth for the sake of the
experiment, and in such studies, liars might not be
aroused enough to show cues of nervousness.
Summarizing the literature, there seem to be a
greater number of reliable verbal cues to deception
than nonverbal cues. This contradicts most police

Detection of Deception: Nonverbal Cues——— 201

D-Cutler (Encyc)-45463.qxd 11/18/2007 12:41 PM Page 201

Free download pdf