eyewitness identification include the following. Many
judges have ruled that the content of the expert testi-
mony is merely a matter of common sense and, there-
fore, of little benefit to jurors. Judges have also ruled
that the research is not commonly accepted within the
relevant discipline. Other judges have ruled that the
testimony is not helpful to jurors, because it addresses
only research and does not inform the jury about the
accuracy of the identification in the specific case at
trial. Still other judges have ruled that the testimony
will prejudice the jury by making them unnecessarily
skeptical about the eyewitness identification.
Psychological research on expert testimony has
taken various forms. Some research has directly
examined the validity of the reasons for excluding the
expert testimony. For example, as noted above, judges
have ruled that the content of the testimony does not
go beyond the jurors’ common sense. This implies
that the average juror is well-informed about the fac-
tors that influence eyewitness identification accuracy.
Scholars have developed several ways of testing the
degree to which jurors are so informed. Some have
conducted surveys of juror knowledge by administer-
ing test questions about factors affecting eyewitness
identification. Others have described eyewitness iden-
tification experiments to students and asked them to
predict the outcomes of the experiments. Their pre-
dicted outcomes are then compared with the actual
outcomes of the experiments. Still other scholars have
developed simulated trials (transcripts or videotaped
enactments of trials) in which some evidence is held
constant while other evidence (e.g., the suggestive-
ness of lineup procedures) is systematically manipu-
lated. This methodology enables the researcher to
examine what factors influence juror decisions and
whether those factors are consistent with what is
known from the research on eyewitness identification.
This body of research on common sense supports the
conclusion that what psychologists have to offer to the
jurors well exceeds their common sense. For example,
contrary to conclusions from the research literature,
eligible jurors lack a full understanding of the impact
of cross-race recognition, high stress, and weapon
focus on identification accuracy and believe that eye-
witness confidence is a stronger predictor of identifi-
cation accuracy than the research suggests.
Research has also examined the extent to which the
content of expert testimony is generally accepted
among psychologists with expertise in eyewitness
identification. This research takes the form of surveys
of these experts and shows that many (but not all) of
the factors about which experts testify are generally
accepted in the relevant scientific community.
Does expert testimony about eyewitness identifica-
tion prejudice the jury? What effect does expert testi-
mony have on juries? Some researchers have addressed
these questions by conducting trial simulation experi-
ments. In these experiments, students or jury-eligible
community members serve as “mock-jurors” and
either read trial transcripts or view videotaped enact-
ments of trials. Some trials would contain expert tes-
timony while others would not. The research findings
are mixed, with many studies showing that expert tes-
timony makes jurors more skeptical about the accu-
racy of eyewitness identification, meaning they are
less likely to convict a defendant based on eyewitness
identification after hearing expert testimony. Other
studies have found that expert testimony improves
juror decision making by teaching jurors to rely on
factors that are known to influence the likelihood of
false identification (e.g., high stress, weapon focus,
suggestive lineup procedures) and to not rely on fac-
tors that do not predict identification accuracy (e.g.,
witness confidence).
In sum, expert psychological testimony about eye-
witness identification is a relatively new safeguard
developed to prevent mistaken identifications from
resulting in erroneous convictions. Its effectiveness is
limited by virtue of the fact that it is often not allowed
in criminal cases. Some of the reasons that judges typ-
ically give for not allowing this form of expert testi-
mony are not supported by empirical research.
Research on the effectiveness of expert testimony sug-
gests that it can be helpful but not universally so.
Brian L. Cutler
See also Expert Psychological Testimony, Admissibility
Standards; Expert Psychological Testimony, Forms of; Expert
Testimony, Qualifications of Experts; Eyewitness Memory
Further Readings
Benton, T. R., Ross, D. F., Bradshaw, E., Thomas, W. N., &
Bradshaw, G. S. (2006). Eyewitness memory is still not
common sense: Comparing jurors, judges and law
enforcement to eyewitness experts. Applied Cognitive
Psychology, 20,115–129.
Cutler, B. L., & Penrod, S. D. (1995).Mistaken
identification: Eyewitnesses, psychology and the law.New
York: Cambridge University Press.
Expert Psychological Testimony on Eyewitness Identification——— 279
E-Cutler (Encyc)-45463.qxd 11/18/2007 12:42 PM Page 279