Encyclopedia of Psychology and Law

(lily) #1
eyewitnesses (aged 17 to 81 years). The witnesses indi-
vidually viewed a video reconstruction of a robbery at
a savings bank. No weapons were seen in the video,
although the culprit indicated to the clerk that he had a
gun. The critical aspect of this video for the purposes of
the study was the length of exposure to the culprit’s
face in the video. Two versions of the video were cre-
ated. In one version, the culprit’s face (full-frontal and
profile view) was visible for 45 seconds, and in another
version, the culprit’s face (full-frontal and profile view)
was visible for 12 seconds. No other details differed,
and the videos were of the same duration (1 minute 40
seconds). About 35 to 40 minutes after witnessing the
robbery, witnesses in the long-exposure group made
more correct identifications of the robber when he was
present in the lineup. They also provided more correct
descriptions of the robber under the long-exposure con-
dition. A longer exposure did not appear to inflate false
identifications when the culprit was absent from the
lineup in the Aberdeen study.
One additional finding from the comparison of wit-
nesses exposed to a target for a shorter or longer dura-
tion in the Aberdeen study could be of use to
investigators. Witnesses in the long-exposure condi-
tion were more confident in their identification deci-
sions than were witnesses in the short-exposure
condition. However, they were confident even when
they were inaccurate. In other words, confidence was
nota reliable indicator of accuracy under long expo-
sure. This effect was most marked in the culprit-absent
conditions. This finding becomes more meaningful
when the implications for assessing witness credibility
are examined. When deciding whether or not a given
witness is likely to be reliable, a police officer or a
juror may rely on that witness’s verbal expression of
their confidence. To summarize, the research suggests
that the likelihood of a witness making an accurate
identification is increased if he or she has seen the per-
petrator’s face for a longer period of time. However, an
extended exposure could make witnesses more confi-
dent in their identification ability even when they are
wrong. Therefore, while a longer exposure increases
the chances of an accurate identification, investigators
should not rely too heavily on witness confidence as an
indicator of accuracy.
So far it has been proposed that the extent of time
of exposure to a face could be useful information when
assessing the potential of an eyewitness to aid an inves-
tigation and the administration of justice. One of the
limitations of prior research on exposure duration is

that it has been assumed that a witness who is exposed
to a perpetrator for a longer time will be paying more
attention to the face and processing it more “deeply,”
thereby providing a stronger and more accessible
memory trace. However, this assumes that there is
nothing else at the scene of the crime to attract one’s
attention. This is typically not the case. For example,
research has shown that when a perpetrator is holding
a weapon, a witness’s attention may be drawn to that,
and consequently, the witness may spend less time
looking at the face (referred to as the weapon-focus
effect). It is important in future research to identify
various situational factors that alter the relationship
between degree of exposure and memory for an event.

Amina Memon

See also Confidence in Identifications; Confidence in
Identifications, Malleability; Eyewitness Memory;
Eyewitness Memory, Lay Beliefs About; Retention
Interval and Eyewitness Memory; Weapon Focus

Further Readings
Memon, A., Hope, L., & Bull, R. H. C. (2003). Exposure
duration: Effects on eyewitness accuracy and confidence.
British Journal of Psychology, 94,339–354.
Pedersen, A., & Wright, A. (2002). Do differences in event
descriptions cause differences in duration estimates?
Applied Cognitive Psychology, 16,769–783.
Read, J. D. (1995). The availability heuristic in person
identification: The sometimes misleading consequences of
enhanced contextual information. Applied Cognitive
Psychology, 9,91–121.

EXTREMEEMOTIONALDISTURBANCE


A number of states in the United States provide
by statute that defendants charged with murder or
attempted murder may seek to mitigate the charges
against them by claiming, and proving, that when they
intentionally murdered or attempted to murder their
victim, they did so under the influence of an extreme
mental or emotional disturbance (EED) for which
there was a reasonable explanation or excuse. Typically,
such statutes provide that the reasonableness of such
explanation or excuse shall be determined from the
viewpoint of a person in the defendant’s situation at
the time of the crime, under the circumstances as the

Extreme Emotional Disturbance——— 283

E-Cutler (Encyc)-45463.qxd 11/18/2007 12:42 PM Page 283

Free download pdf