changes in appearance make accurate recognition sig-
nificantly more difficult. This decrease in recognition
can be dramatic depending on the degree of change.
The greater the change, the greater the decrease in
accuracy for witnesses, both adults and children, trying
to make an identification. The hair, hairline, and upper
portion of the face, if obscured, are particularly ineffec-
tive for later accurate identification. Both the simulta-
neous and sequential lineup procedures have been
tested in laboratory settings to determine their efficacy
when a culprit’s appearance has changed (e.g., hair-
style, facial hair). For child and adult witnesses, both
lineup procedures produced comparable and lowered
accurate identification rates when an appearance
change occurred compared with the case when there
was no change.
Remembering Faces
How do we remember a face? Do we remember the
features of a face or do we remember the whole face
as a gestalt? Some debate has occurred over this issue,
with a number of questions remaining unanswered. It
may be that both types of encoding occur or that one
strategy is more relied on depending on the develop-
mental stage of the observer. For example, it has been
suggested that adolescents and adults are more likely
to use a gestalt or holistic approach to remembering
faces, taking the whole face in, whereas younger
children may be more likely to rely on a featural strat-
egy, focusing on individual features.
Change of Appearance:
Facial Characteristics
Regardless of the process that we use to remember a
face, it becomes much more difficult to do this when
facial characteristics change. Moreover, a change in
one feature may make the whole face appear different.
Consider the case when someone changes hairstyles
or hair color or if a male shaves off his beard or grows
facial hair. Changes in any of these features make it
more difficult to correctly recognize that person.
The influence of three facial changes on recogni-
tion/identification accuracy has been examined across
a number of studies: changes in hairstyle, facial hair,
and the addition or removal of glasses. To study
the influence of these changes, often participants
are presented with several photographs featuring dif-
ferent “targets.” Following some delay, participants
are presented with another set of photographs, some
of which are never-before-seen faces, some are of the
targets as they appeared in the initial set of pho-
tographs, and others are of the targets but with some
changes in appearance—for example, the target may
not be wearing glasses in the first set of photos but
could be wearing glasses in the new set. When an
alteration or change is made, there is a significant
decrease in accurate identification. Moreover, when
changes to facial features are combined, the difficulty
with identification can increase. Most often, changes
to facial features results in an inability to correctly
recognize the person seen previously.
The natural aging process can also make accurate
identification more difficult. For example, a 2-year
difference in time can reduce recognizability, in par-
ticular if there is a large discrepancy between the two
appearances, such as when facial hair is grown. In one
study examining the aging process, participants ini-
tially viewed photographs of high school students
whom they would later have to recognize in a set of
photos taken 2 years later. Participants had difficulty
in correctly recognizing photos if there was a large
discrepancy between the high school photo and the
photo taken of the same person 2 years later.
Disguise
Culprits may attempt to evade identification by wearing
a variety of disguises that conceal either part or most of
their face during the commission of the crime. They
have been known to wear ski masks revealing just the
eyes and mouth, hats and sunglasses, stockings over
their head, and other sorts of masks. Studies that have
examined the influence of disguise often have partici-
pants watch a videotaped mock crime in which half the
participants see the culprit wearing a disguise, such as
a knit cap that obscures the hair and hairline. The
remaining participants see the culprit without the dis-
guise. Research indicates that participants are almost
twice as likely to provide an accurate identification of
the culprit when there is no disguise than when a dis-
guise is donned. Moreover, the facial composites pro-
duced by participants who see a culprit with a cap show
much greater variability than composites from those
participants who saw a culprit without a cap.
Researchers have attempted to determine which
features are more essential for later recognition/
identification. The upper portions of the face, includ-
ing the hair, hairline, and eyes, seem more critical for
later accurate recognition than the lower portions.
Hair in particular is a feature that many people focus
288 ———Eyewitness Identification: Effect of Disguises and Appearance Changes
E-Cutler (Encyc)-45463.qxd 11/18/2007 12:42 PM Page 288