eyewitness, and the tendency of eyewitnesses to choose
suspects from a lineup previously encountered in mug
shot arrays. Other factors agreed on by a majority of
experts included the impact of alcohol on the eyewit-
ness, the difficulty in making identifications of perpe-
trators not of the same race as the eyewitness, and the
reduction in false identifications due to the use of
sequential rather than simultaneous lineups. Other phe-
nomena that were supported by at least two thirds of the
experts included the inferior accuracy of the child eye-
witness when compared with the adult eyewitness, the
potential for misleading memories recovered from
childhood, and how the use of similar foils (here foil
refers to an innocent person in a police lineup) in a
lineup increases eyewitness accuracy.
The Opinions of Judges
Although individual jurors are ultimately responsible
for interpreting the testimony of an expert witness and
applying their insight to the facts of the case at hand,
judges alone determine whether the expert witness
meets the Daubert criteria for inclusion in the trial.
Judges, like jurors, may rely on common sense when
interpreting eyewitness evidence in the absence of for-
mal psychological training. Eyewitness identification
errors have been cited in many cases of wrongful con-
viction, although a survey reported that only 43% of
judges believed that such errors have been made in
half of the reported cases of wrongful conviction.
However, not all evidence is discouraging. Survey
data on judges’ knowledge and beliefs about eyewit-
ness factors revealed that while judges may agree cor-
rectly with statements on eyewitness issues, these
same individuals report that the average juror would
not be likely to respond correctly. A modest correla-
tion (r=.30) was reported between a judge’s knowl-
edge of eyewitness factors and the willingness of the
judge to admit expert testimony.
Agreement between judges and experts was observed
on less than half (40%) of 30 eyewitness factors, which
included (but were not limited to) the role of attitudes
and expectations, the cross-race effect, and the impact
of exposure time. Judges were in agreement with
experts on less than half (37%) of the listed system
variables, including the malleability of an eyewitness’s
confidence, the biasing effects of showup lineups, and
what constitutes a fair lineup. Judges were not in
agreement with experts regarding the phenomenon of
hypnotic suggestion. When data collected from a 2004
survey of judges were compared with the results of
experts in a previous survey, judges and experts agreed
on 7 of 8 items.
The Opinions of Jurors
If a case is tried by a jury, the jurors serve as the ulti-
mate arbiter of fact in the courtroom, and they must
decide not only on whether case-relevant eyewitness
factors should be taken into consideration during delib-
eration but also on what weight should be given when
considering a verdict. The testimony of experts may
serve to allay juror concerns about eyewitness phenom-
ena. Nearly three quarters of respondents in one survey
replied that their primary aim as an expert witness was
to educate the jury. Thankfully, few researchers reported
that they would be willing to testify in court regarding
an eyewitness factor on which the published literature
was “inconclusive.” In contrast, approximately three
quarters of those experts who regarded the evidence as
“generally reliable” and a plurality of those who saw it
as “very reliable” were willing to testify about these
factors. Ninety-five percent of these surveyed experts
believed that expert testimony on eyewitness issues had
a positive impact on juries.
There was correspondence between experts and
jurors on only 4 of the 30 survey statements (13%). As
expected, significant differences in the rates of agree-
ment emerged between experts and jurors on the
statements focusing on factors classified as system vari-
ables, such that jurors were less in agreement about
the eyewitness factor than the experts. The four eye-
witness factors that experts and jurors did agree on
were statements regarding the effects that violence,
alcohol, and stress have on an eyewitness and the fact
that trained observers are not more accurate witnesses
than untrained people. The largest discrepancies
observed between experts and jurors were found for
statements regarding lineup instructions and hypnotic
suggestibility, with jurors expressing significantly
less agreement about those eyewitness factors than
experts. Juror accuracy (50.7%) differed significantly
from the level of accuracy seen among judges and law
enforcement personnel when accuracy was defined as
agreement with those statements to which at least
75% of experts agree.
Other Evidence
Understanding the general acceptance of eyewitness
factors among law enforcement personnel is criti-
cal in that members of this population draw their
Eyewitness Identification: General Acceptance in the Scientific Community——— 293
E-Cutler (Encyc)-45463.qxd 11/18/2007 12:42 PM Page 293