Encyclopedia of Psychology and Law

(lily) #1
into their decisions when they are presented with a
case description that includes the relevant eyewitness
factors (e.g., cross-race effect). In one of the few
investigations of this question, Brian Cutler and col-
leagues found that even when jurors had specific
knowledge of the limitations of eyewitness identifica-
tion, the information was not well integrated into their
decision making. Similarly, other researchers have
recently found that mock jurors who have demonstra-
bly more knowledge than others do not necessarily
demonstrate sensitivity to the eyewitness factors rele-
vant to a case. In summary, researchers have con-
cluded that there is little evidence that the existing
knowledge held by mock jurors is readily incorpo-
rated into their decisions regarding a written vignette.
Finally, Brian Cutler and colleagues have also
attempted to improve levels of mock juror knowledge
through the presentation of expert testimony prior to
making judgments about cases in which eyewitness
identification factors are manipulated. This research
has been completed in laboratory settings with mock
jurors, and as a result, its generalizability to court-
rooms and jury deliberations is unknown. Nonetheless,
these studies suggest that whereas the presentation of
relevant expert testimony may increase low levels of
juror knowledge or awareness of relevant eyewitness
factors, the integration of this knowledge into juror
decision making may or may not be successful,
depending on the particular variables of interest. Thus,
if expert testimony is recommended as a safeguard
against weak juror understanding of eyewitness fac-
tors, it does not appear to be particularly effective.

Potential Difficulties With
Evaluations of Lay Knowledge
A number of issues are relevant to the reliability and
validity of the kinds of assessments of lay belief
described above. First, in a temporal sense, public sur-
vey results have limited validity because public beliefs
and knowledge will change over time. These changes
likely result from improved scientific understanding
and its dissemination to the general public through var-
ious media and by integration into formal education.
Second, considerably more research is required to
determine the extent to which survey and mock trial
responses accurately reflect the beliefs of jury-eligible
participants. This issue concerns the sensitivity and
reliability of the various assessment procedures
described above and the extent to which lay responses

may be directly compared with those of experts. For
example, even with ostensibly identical foci of
the questions posed to experts and the public, the
response options provided have not been identical.
Similarly, if statements are written by experts and
offered without change to survey participants, on what
basis can we argue that the public understands the
statements in a manner similar to that of the experts?
Furthermore, the translation of the expert items into
meaningful statements for lay respondents is difficult
and suggests that real understanding of these issues by
jurors (and by judges, trial counsel, and experts alike)
will only be gained with more in-depth interviews,
open-ended questions, and the use of techniques that
can assess response consistency within individuals
across both question formats and time.
A third question is whether the samples surveyed to
date actually represent the members of a population of
individuals who may be called for jury duty and who
serve as jurors. Many studies have relied on undergrad-
uate students, albeit jury-eligible in most cases, but who
arguably are not representative of actual jurors: In fact,
university students infrequently serve on actual juries.
Additionally, even those studies in which community
samples were included, nonetheless, suffer from weak
representativeness because there may be important
demographic and attitudinal differences between com-
munity members who, once called, appear versus those
who fail to appear for jury duty. A more compelling
approach would be to collect data from actual jurors
who have participated in trials or to survey community
members who have been called and appear for jury duty
but have yet to be assigned to a particular case.
In summary, a fairly consistent description of juror
knowledge emerges across a wide variety of assess-
ment methods; specifically, jurors appear to have lim-
ited understanding of eyewitness issues and research
findings.

J. Don Read and Sarah L. Desmarais

See also Cognitive Interview; Confidence in Identifications;
Cross-Race Effect in Eyewitness Identification; Elderly
Eyewitnesses; Estimator and System Variables in
Eyewitness Identification; Expert Psychological
Testimony; Expert Psychological Testimony on
Eyewitness Identification; Exposure Time and Eyewitness
Memory; Eyewitness Memory; Instructions to the
Witness; Retention Interval and Eyewitness Memory;
Simultaneous and Sequential Lineup Presentation;
Weapon Focus

302 ———Eyewitness Memory, Lay Beliefs About

E-Cutler (Encyc)-45463.qxd 11/18/2007 12:42 PM Page 302

Free download pdf