Encyclopedia of Psychology and Law

(lily) #1
example, by pressing eyewitnesses to answer ques-
tions about events that they are unsure of. Kathy
Pezdek and her colleagues conducted several studies
to examine this issue. In this study, individuals viewed
a crime video and then answered open-ended ques-
tions that included answerable and unanswerable
questions about the video. Half the participants were
in the “spontaneous guess” condition; the “Don’t
know” response option was available to them, so they
did not need to guess any answers. The other half of
the participants were in the “forced guess” condition
and did not have a “Don’t know” response option.
One week later, the same questions were answered
with a “Don’t know” option available for everyone.
The primary finding concerns the following ques-
tion: If participants were forced to guess answers to
unanswerablequestions at Time 1, were the answers
they generated likely to be recalled 1 week later at Time
2, when they all had the option of responding, “Don’t
know”? The responses to unanswerable questions are
the most revealing in this study, because we know that
the individuals did not actually observe the information
relevant to answering those questions. The mean pro-
portion of responses that received the same answer at
Time 1 and Time 2 was significantly higher in the spon-
taneous guess condition (M=.54) than in the forced
guess condition (M=.40). This result suggests that
although false confabulation does occur, false informa-
tion that resulted from forced confabulation is less
likely to persist in memory than false information
that individuals spontaneously provided because they
thought they had observed it. Furthermore, when the
same answer was given to an unanswerable question
both times, the confidence expressed in the answer
increased over time both for answers that were sponta-
neously guessed and those that were forced guesses.
Thus, erroneous memories that occur from self-
generated false confabulation are confidently held. This
is of course problematic from the point of view of
assessing the veracity of eyewitness memories because
it suggests that it may be difficult to differentiate
between true and falsely confabulated memories.
This topic is relevant to the specialty of psychology
and law because virtually 100% of all eyewitnesses to
crimes who eventually testify in court are interviewed
by police officers at least once, and typically multiple
times. Forced confabulation can occur in police inter-
views when officers press an eyewitness to answer a
question even though the eyewitness has indicated
that he or she does not know or is unsure of the answer

to the question. In addition, police interrogation typi-
cally involves techniques to pressure witnesses to
answer questions they are reluctant or unable to
answer. It is important to recognize that such tech-
niques are likely to generate forced confabulations—
even confidently held forced confabulations—as well
as true information. Although no data exist document-
ing how frequently this practice occurs in real police
interviews, Richard Leo has reported that this is not an
unusual practice, that, in fact, forced confessions
commonly occur under these circumstances.

Kathy Pezdek

See alsoDelusions; Eyewitness Memory; False Confessions;
False Memories; Postevent Information and Eyewitness
Memory

Further Readings
Ackil, J. K., & Zaragoza, M. S. (1998). Memorial
consequences of forced confabulation: Age differences in
susceptibility to false memories. Developmental
Psychology, 34,1358–1372.
Leo, R. A. (1996). Inside the interrogation room. Journal of
Criminal Law and Criminology, 86,266–303.
Pezdek, K., Sperry, K., & Owens, S. (in press). Interviewing
witnesses: The effect of forced confabulation on event
memory. Law & Human Behavior.

FORCIBLEMEDICATION


This entry discusses the involuntary administration of
psychotropic medication, which continues to be one
of the most controversial issues in mental health law.
Whether mental patients in hospital, the community,
jail, prison, or the judicial process may refuse psy-
chotropic medication that the government would like
to administer raises complex legal, clinical, moral,
and social issues. Psychotropic medication is by far
the leading treatment technique for patients diagnosed
with mental illness. Although demonstrably helpful
for many patients, it often imposes serious direct,
often debilitating, and unwanted side effects that are
beyond the patient’s ability to resist and that may be
long lasting. As a result, involuntary administration of
these drugs raises serious constitutional questions.
Most states now have statutory and administrative
restrictions on involuntary treatment. The limits

Forcible Medication——— 325

F-Cutler (Encyc)-45463.qxd 11/18/2007 12:42 PM Page 325

Free download pdf