Wason, P. C., and P. N. Johnson-Laird. (1972). The psychology of reasoning: Struc-
ture and content. London: Batsford.
Watson, J. (1982). Of flesh and bones: The management of death pollution in
Cantonese society. In M. Bloch and J. Parry (eds.), Death and the regeneration of life
(pp. 155–186). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Watts, I. (1999). The origin of symbolic culture. In R. Dunbar, C. Knight, and C.
Power (eds.), The evolution of culture (pp. 113–146). New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers Uni-
versity Press.
Wetzel, C. G., and M. D. Walton. (1985). Developing biased social judgments:
The false-consensus effect. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 49(5), 1352–1359.
Whitehouse, H. (1995). Inside the cult: Religious innovation and transmission in
Papua, New Guinea. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
_____. (2000).Arguments and icons: Divergent modes of religiosity. Oxford: Oxford [357]
University Press.
Williams, G. (1966). Adaptation and natural selection: A critique of some current evolu-
tionary thought. Princeton: Princeton University Press.
Wilson, J. Q. (1993). The moral sense. New York: Free Press.
Wimmer, H., S. Gruber, and J. Perner. (1984). Young children's conception of
lying: Lexical realism—moral subjectivism. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology,
37(1), 1–30.
Wimmer, H., and J. Perner. (1983). Beliefs about beliefs: Representation and con-
straining function of wrong beliefs in young children's understanding of deception.
Cognition, 13, 103–128.
Wolpert, L. (1992). The unnatural nature of science. London: Faber and Faber.
Wright, R. (1994). The moral animal: Evolutionary psychology and everyday life. New
York: Random House.
Wynn, K. (1990). Children's understanding of counting. Cognition,36, 155–193.
REFERENCES