22 January 2022 | New Scientist | 45
It is hard to forget the anxiety
of early 2020, as the covid-19
pandemic swept the world.
There was the fear of the
disease, of course, but also
serious concerns about our
overall well-being. Even if we
and our loved ones survived the
illness, how could we cope with
the isolation and uncertainty?
The pandemic has clearly
been a terrible trauma for many
people – and their experiences
mustn’t be forgotten. On
average, however, people
appear to have been remarkably
resilient. According to the latest
World Happiness Report
published in March 2021,
measures of life satisfaction in
the UK only fell modestly on a
10-point score, from 7.16 in
2019 to 6.8 in 2020. In the US,
scores rose, from 6.94 to 7.03.
These small shifts seemed to
be confirmed by other surveys,
suggesting that it wasn’t simply
an artefact of the particular
methods used.
There is no easy explanation
for this resilience. Richard
Layard, who has been co-author
of the World Happiness Reports
from their inception, thinks it
comes down to the role of social
comparison in shaping our
happiness. In general, our life
satisfaction is much more
dependent on relative changes
in our fortunes compared with
other individuals, rather than
general shifts in the population
as a whole (see main story). In
this case, the stresses have felt
less damaging because of the
knowledge that other people
were undergoing the same
struggles. “I think this sense
of a common threat was
important,” says Layard.
COVID-19 VS
HAPPINESS
When you account for this possibility and
calculate the benefits that citizens actually
receive instead of the initial investment, it
becomes clear that an effective welfare state
really does play an important role in people’s
life satisfaction. Overall, the difference in life
satisfaction between the best and worst
welfare states was roughly equivalent to the
effects of marriage on someone’s happiness.
Intriguingly, the increased well-being could be
seen at all levels of society, including those
with the highest incomes.
Nordic nirvana
The Nordic countries – and Finland in
particular – provide a perfect illustration of all
these trends. They each have low economic
inequality, high levels of social trust and an
effective welfare state. In Martela’s view, all
three of these factors have interacted to create
a kind of virtuous cycle. The welfare state has
helped reduce inequality, which in turn has
helped increase people’s sense of trust in
others. Equally, Finnish people’s trust in each
other and the quality of their institutions have
meant that they are more willing to see their
taxes go into the welfare state. “I recall one
American economist who said that Nordic
countries are the only places in the world
where you can go into elections saying ‘I’m
going to raise taxes’ and still win,” says Martela.
An examination of the Nordic nations can
also help us dispel some lingering myths.
Many commentators, for instance, have
expressed surprise that Finns could be so
satisfied with their lives when the population
has to endure such cold, dark winters. But
reams of scientific research show that climate
has very little influence on life satisfaction
in the long term. This seems to be the result
of habituation: while a single rainy or sunny
day might affect our mood, we soon adapt
to long-term weather patterns.
An equally persistent myth concerns mental
health. Nordic countries are commonly
believed to have unusually high numbers
of suicides. If that were true, it might suggest
that the happiness of the majority comes at a
terrible cost to the minority who fail to live up
to the nations’ high standards of success and
well-being, and who may be left feeling
“ AN EFFECTIVE
WELFARE
S TAT E R E A L LY
DOES PLAY AN
IMPORTANT
ROLE IN
PEOPLE’S LIFE
SATISFACTION”
>
TAR
A^ M
OO
RE
/GE
TT
Y^ I
MA
GE
S;^ M
AT
T^ D
AR
TF
OR
D