The Dictionary of Human Geography

(nextflipdebug2) #1

Comp. by: VPugazhenthi Stage : Revises1 ChapterID: 9781405132879_4_M Date:1/4/
09 Time:15:19:38 Filepath:H:/00_Blackwell/00_3B2/Gregory-9781405132879/appln/3B2/
revises/9781405132879_4_M.3d


are what make the Hispanic community in
this region of the USA cohere as a social and
political force. The same could be argued
for Palestinians or, in Canada, First Nations
groups. In diasporic and post-colonial con-
texts – in which memory is threatened by
both nostalgia and coerced assimilation – ‘cultu-
ral memory offers promise of epistemological
grounding’, though not necessarily within a
singular nationalidentity(Sugg, 2003, p. 469:
see alsodiaspora;post-colonialism;transna-
tionalism).Counter-memoriesmay beassembled
and transmitted through oral tradition, but also
in less bureaucratized time-places: thebody,
domestic spaces (Blunt, 2003), neighbourhoods
or ‘temporal re-territorializations’ of formal
spaces (such as carnivals, festivals or rallies;
Legg, 2005).
The memory projects of marginalized
groups may bear the traces of trauma, such
that the possibilities of memory are altered.
With traumatic recall, events remain in the
vivid present, resisting integration through
narrativization. Though thestateoften in-
corporates violent or tragic events into a linear
narrative of national redemption and over-
coming, what Edkins (2003) calls ‘trauma
time’ works differently, and its repetitive dis-
ruptive quality can reveal the violent founda-
tions ofsovereign power. Trauma thus has a
relation not just totimebut alsospaceand
geography; for instance, to narrations and
experiences of nation and persistent claims to
homeland. Sugg (2003) draws on Hirsch’s
concept of post-memory to understand the
‘suspended migration’ of second-generation
Cuban Americans:childrenof exiled parents
may inherit the collective cultural trauma of
their parents and remember their parents’
stories of exile as their own within a dynamic
of longing and return. Alternatively, memor-
ializing trauma in the landscape may consti-
tute a witnessing public, setting in motion an
emerging narrative (and a potential release
from traumatic recall; Burk, forthcoming).
The recent tendency has been to expand the
scope of memory studies by considering the
role ofperformanceandbodilyandnon-
bodily practices in the making of memorial
landscapes (Hoelscher, 2003), by examining
the wider production of social memory beyond
demarcated sites of monuments and memor-
ials, and by considering the landscape impli-
cations of the memories ofanimalsor other
than human beings (Lorimer, 2006). nj/gp

Suggested reading
Johnson(2003b,2005);Legg(2007a);Till(2003).

mental maps/cognitive maps Perhaps the
best-known research outcome frombehav-
ioural geographywas the retrieval of the
imagined or mental maps widespread in the
popular knowledge of places, mental con-
structs that were seen as intervening between
geographical settings and human action. An
early study was the simple sketch mapping of
urban areas from memory supervised by Kevin
Lynch in the pursuit of good urban design,
which permitted animage of the cityto be con-
structed, revealing districts of knowledge and
ignorance, and the role of such remembered
features as nodes, edges and landmarks in
establishing urban legibility. Behavioural geo-
graphers, including Roger Downs and David
Stea (1973), in contrast referred tocognitive
maps, which they associated with the spatial
tasks of orientation and way-finding. More
formal and widely replicated were the experi-
ments with paper and pencil tests conducted by
Peter Gould and his students (Gould and
White, 1993 [1974]), which were intended
not so much to identifyplaceknowledge and
place ignorance but, rather, to establish a sur-
face of place preferences. From surveys in sev-
eral countries, mental maps were constructed
that revealed both a national preference surface
and also a local surface of desirability for a
home area. Subsequent work sought to estab-
lish the developmental growth ofmapsamong
childrenof increasing age, and examined
linkages between geographical preference
surfaces and future residential choice and
migrationpropensities (Gould and White,
1993 [1974]).
Mental maps were part of a broader move-
ment inenvironmental perception, which in
turn has elided into an interest in therepre-
sentationandsocial constructionof places
in a variety of disciplines using lesspositivist
methods and emphasizing social rather than
psychological factors. Nonetheless, the older
analytical methods continue to generate inter-
esting results (Kitchin, 1994), even if with
interdisciplinary dissemination the links with
the original work are truncated or forgotten.
So a current study of the role of themediain
shaping the spatial surface of fear in Los
Angeles (Matei and Ball-Rokeach, 2005),
contains the key words mental maps, GIS
and spatial effects, but omits any reference to
Gould’s work, including his celebrated feature
inTimemagazine that included a map of the
perceived fear of urban areas. dl

Suggested reading
Gould and White (1993).

Gregory / The Dictionary of Human Geography 9781405132879_4_M Final Proof page 455 1.4.2009 3:19pm

MENTAL MAPS/COGNITIVE MAPS
Free download pdf