Cognitive Therapy of Anxiety Disorders

(sharon) #1

52 COGNITIVE THEORY AND RESEARCH ON ANXIETY


normal anD abnormal anxiety: a Cognitive perspeCtive

In our description of the cognitive model, we focused primarily on pathological anxiety.
As noted earlier, fear can be adaptive and anxiety is a common experience in every-
day life. So, how does the cognitive model explain the difference between normal and
abnormal anxiety? This is an important consideration for clinical practitioners as well
as researchers. After all, our goal as therapists is to normalize the experience of anxiety.
Thus what is the nature of normal cognitive processing of anxiety? Table 2.5 sum-
marizes a few key differences at the automatic and elaborative phases of information
processing that characterize nonclinical and clinical anxiety.


Automatic Cognitive Processes in Normal Anxiety


Given the automatic and involuntary nature of the immediate fear response, it is obvious
that individuals who do not suffer an anxiety disorder have a distinct advantage over
clinical samples. In normal anxiety, the orienting mode is not as primed for the detec-
tion of negative self- referent stimuli as in the anxiety disorders. In nonclinical states,
the detection of negative stimuli will still be given attentional priority, but the range of
stimuli that would be identified as negative and potentially self- relevant would be nar-
rower. In fact, Mogg and Bradley (1999a) reviewed evidence that less anxious individu-
als show attentional avoidance of low threat stimuli whereas highly anxious individuals
show enhanced attention to low, and especially moderately, threatening stimuli (see also


table 2.5. Cognitive Differences between normal and abnormal anxiety predicted by the
Cognitive model
Phase of processing Abnormal anxiety Normal anxiety


Orienting mode ••Heightened sensitivity to negative
stimuli


••More balanced sensitivity to the
detection of positive and negative
stimuli

Primal threat activation ••Exaggerated primary appraisal of
threat
••Negative evaluation of autonomic
arousal
••Presence of threat-related
processing biases and errors
••Frequent and salient automatic
thoughts and images of threat
••Initiation of automatic, inhibitory
self-protective behaviors


••More appropriate, reality-based
appraisal of threat
••Views arousal as an uncomfortable
but not a threatening state
••Attention not as narrowly focused
on threat; fewer cognitive errors
••Fewer and less salient anxious
thoughts and images
••Delay in inhibitory self-protective
behaviors as more elaborative
coping responses are considered

Secondary elaborative
reappraisal


••Focus on weakness; low self-
efficacy and negative outcome
expectancy
••Poor processing of safety cues
••Inaccessibility of constructive mode
thinking
••Uncontrollable, threat-oriented
worry
••Initial threat estimation is enhanced

••Focus on strength; high self-efficacy
and positive outcome expectancy
••Better processing of safety cues
••Ability to access and utilize
constructive mode thinking
••More controlled and reflective,
problem-oriented worry
••Initial threat estimation is
diminished
Free download pdf