118 Foundations of Visual Perception
Lindsay, P. H., & Norman, D. A. (1977).Human information process-
ing: An introduction to psychology.New York: Academic Press.
Link, S. W. (1992). The wave theory of difference and similarity.
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Luce, R. D., & Krumhansl, C. L. (1988). Measurement, scaling, and
psychophysics. In R. C. Atkinson, R. J. Herrnstein, & G. Lindzey
(Eds.),Stevens’ handbook of experimental psychology(2nd ed.,
pp. 3–74). New York: Wiley.
MacKay, D. M. (1986). Vision: The capture of optical covariation.
In J. D. Pettigrew, K. J. Sanderson, & W. R. Levick (Eds.), Visual
neuroscience (pp. 365–373). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge
University Press.
Macmillan, N. A., & Creelman, C. D. (1991). Detection theory: A
user’s guide.Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Mamassian, P., Landy, M., & Maloney, L. T. (in press). Bayesian
modeling of visual perception. In R. Rao, B. Olshausen, & M.
Lewicki (Eds.), Probabilistic models of the brain and neural
function.Cambridge, UK: MIT Press.
Marley, A. A. (1989a). A random utility family that includes many of
the “classical” models and has closed form choice probabilities
and choice reaction times.British Journal of Mathematical &
Statistical Psychology, 42,13–36.
Marley, A. A. (1989b). A random utility family that includes many
of the “classical” models and has closed form choice probabili-
ties and choice reaction times: Addendum. British Journal of
Mathematical & Statistical Psychology, 42,280.
Marr, D. (1982). Vision.San Francisco: Freeman.
Mersch, P. P. A., Middendorp, H. M., Bouhuys, A. L., Beersma,
D. G. M., & Hoofdakker, R. H. van den. (1999). Seasonal affec-
tive disorder and latitude: A review of the literature. Journal of
Affective Disorders, 53,35–48.
Metz, C. E. (1998). Roc analysis.Retrieved August 14, 2001, from
http://www-radiology.uchicago.edu/krl/toppage11.htm.
Milner, A. D., & Goodale, M. A. (1995). The visual brain in action.
Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
Nakayama, K. (1994). Gibson: An appreciation. Psychological
Review, 101,329–335.
Nakayama, K. (1998). Vision fin de siècle: A reductionistic explana-
tion of perception for the 21st century. In J. Hochberg (Ed.),
Perception and cognition at century’s end(pp. 307–331). San
Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Näsänen, R., Ojanpää, H., & Kojo, I. (2001). Effect of stimulus con-
trast on performance and eye movements in visual research.
Vision Research, 41,1817–1824.
Neisser, U. (1967). Cognitive psychology.New York: Appleton-
Century-Crofts.
Nickerson, R. S. (1998). Confirmation bias: A ubiquitous phenome-
non in many guises. Review of General Psychology, 2,175–220.
Norman, J. (in press). Two visual systems and two theories of per-
ception: An attempt to reconcile the constructivist and ecological
approaches.Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
Palmer, S. E. (1976). Fundamental aspects of cognitive representa-
tion. In E. Rosch & B. B. Lloyd (Eds.), Cognition and catego-
rization(pp. 259–303). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Parker, A. J., & Newsome, W. T. (1998). Sense and single neuron:
Probing the physiology of perception. Annual Review of Neuro-
science, 21,227–277.
Pylyshyn, Z. (1984). Computation and cognition.Cambridge, MA:
MIT Press.
Pylyshyn, Z. (1999). Is vision continuous with cognition? The case
for cognitive impenetrability of visual perception. Behavioral
and Brain Sciences, 22,341–365.
Quick, R. F. (1974). A vector magnitude model of contrast detection.
Kybernetik, 16,65–67.
Ramachandran, V. S. (1990a). Interactions between motion, depth,
color, and form: A utilitarian theory of perception. In C.
Blakemore (Ed.), Vision: Coding and efficiency(pp. 346–360).
Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Ramachandran, V. S. (1990b). Visual perception in people and
machines. In R. Blake & T. Troscianko (Eds.), AI and the eye
(pp. 12–77). New York: Wiley.
Rock, I. (1983). The logic of perception.Cambridge, MA: Bradford
Books/MIT Press.
Rock, I. (1997). Indirect perception.Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Rogers, S. (2000). The emerging concept of information. Ecological
Psychology, 12,365–375.
Ruderman, D. L. (1997). Origins of scaling in natural images. Vision
Research, 37,3385–3395.
Rumelhart, D. E. (1977). Introduction to human information
processing.New York: Wiley.
Schneider, G. E. (1969). Two visual systems: Brain mechanisms for
localization and discrimination are dissociated by tectal and
cortical lesions. Science, 63,895–902.
Shannon, C. E., & Weaver, W. (1949). The mathematical theory of
communication.Urbana, IL: University Illinois Press.
Stanislaw, H., & Todorov, N. (1999). Calculation of signal detection
theory measures. Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers,
31,137–149.
Swets, J. A. (1996). Signal detection theory and ROC analysis in
psychology and diagnostics: Collected papers.Hillsdale, NJ:
Erlbaum.
Tanner, W. P., & Sorkin, R. D. (1972). The theory of signal de-
tectability. In J. V. Tobias (Ed.), Foundations of modern auditory
theory(pp. 65–98). New York: Academic Press.
Terman, J. S., & Terman, M. (1999). Photopic and scotopic light de-
tection in patients with seasonal affective disorder and control
subjects.Biological Psychiatry, 46,1642–1648.
Thurstone, L. L. (1928). The phi-gamma hypothesis. Journal of
Experimental Psychology, 11,293–305.
Turvey, M. T., Shaw, R. E., Reid, E. S., & Mace, W. (1981). Eco-
logical laws of perceiving and acting: In reply to Fodor and
Pylyshyn.Cognition, 9,237–304.