Handbook of Psychology, Volume 4: Experimental Psychology

(Axel Boer) #1

176 Touch


Vogels, I. M. L. C., Kappers, A. M. L., & Koenderink, J. J. (1996).
Haptic after-effect of curved surfaces. Perception, 25,
109–119.
Watkins, M. J., & Watkins, O. C. (1974). A tactile suffix effect.
Memory & Cognition, 5,529–534.
Weber, E. H. (1978). The sense of touch(H. E. Ross, Ed. &
Trans.). London: Academic Press. (Original work published
1834)
Weinstein, S. (1968). Intensive and extensive aspects of tactile
sensitivity as a function of body part, sex, and laterality. In D. R.
Kenshalo (Ed.), The skin senses(pp. 195–222). Springfield, IL:
Thomas.
Welch, R. B. & Warren, D. H. (1980). Immediate perceptual
response to intersensory discordance. Psychological Bulletin,
88,638–667.


Wiedel, J. (Ed.). (1983). Proceedings of the first international
conference on maps and graphics for the visually impaired.
Washington, DC: Association of American Geographers.
Wilska, A. (1954). On the vibrational sensitivity in different regions
of the body surface. Acta Psychologica Scandinavia, 31,285–289.
Wolfe, H. K. (1898). Some effects of size on judgments of weight.
Psychological Review, 5,25–54.
Wong, T. S. (1977). Dynamic properties of radial and tangential
movements as determinants of the haptic horizontal-vertical illu-
sion with an L figure.Journal of Experimental Psychology:
Human Perception and Performance, 3,151–164.
Yoshioka, T., Gibb, B., Dorsch, A. K., Hsiao, S. S., & Johnson,
K. O. (2001). Neural coding mechanisms underlying perceived
roughness of finely textured surfaces. Journal of Neuroscience,
21,6905–6916.
Free download pdf