Handbook of Psychology, Volume 4: Experimental Psychology

(Axel Boer) #1

236 Depth Perception and the Perception of Events


Todd, J. T., Reichel, F. D. (1989). Ordinal structure in the visual per-
ception and cognition of smoothly curved surfaces. Psychologi-
cal Review, 96,643–657.
Todd, J. T., & Warren, W. H., Jr. (1982). Visual perception of rela-
tive mass in dynamic events. Perception, 11,325–335.
Tresilian, J. R. (1991). Approximate information sources and
perceptual variables in interceptive timing. Journal of Experi-
mental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20,
154–173.
Tresilian, J. R. (1999). Analysis of recent empirical challenges to an
account of interceptive timing. Perception & Psychophysics, 61,
515–528.
Ullman, S. (1979). The interpretation of visual motion. Cambridge,
MA: MIT Press.
van den Berg, A. V. (1992). Robustness of perception of heading
from optic flow. Vision Research, 32, 1285–1296.
van den Berg, A. V., & Brenner, E. (1994). Humans combine the
optic flow with static depth cues for robust perception of head-
ing.Vision Research, 34,2153–2167.
Vicario, G. B., & Bressan, P. (1990). Wheels: A new illusion in the
perception of rolling objects. Perception, 19,57–61.
Wallach, H., & O’Connell, D. N. (1953). The kinetic depth effect.
Journal of Experimental Psychology, 45, 205–217.
Wann, J. P. (1996). Anticipating arrival: Is the tau-margin a specious
theory?Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Percep-
tion and Performance, 22,1031–1048.
Warren, W. H. (1995). Self-motion: Visual perception and visual
control. In W. Epstein & S. Rogers (Eds.), Perception of space
and motion(pp. 263–325). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Warren, W. H. (1998). The state of flow. In T. Watanabe (Ed.), High-
level motion processing: Computational, neurobiological, and
psychophysical perspectives(pp. 315–358). Cambridge, MA:
MIT Press.
Warren, W. H., Kay, B. A., & Yilmaz, E. H. (1996). Visual control
of posture during walking: Functional specificity. Journal of


Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance,
22,818–838.
Warren, W. H., & Kurtz, K. J. (1992). The role of central and pe-
ripheral vision in perceiving the direction of self-motion. Per-
ception & Psychophysics, 51, 443–454.
Webb, J. A., & Aggarwal, J. K. (1982). Structure from motion of
rigid and jointed objects. Artificial Intelligence, 19,107–130.
Wertheim, A. H. (1994). Motion perception during self-motion: The
direct versus inferential controversy revisited. Behavioral &
Brain Sciences, 17,293–355.
Wertheimer, M. (1937). Laws of organization in perceptual forms. In
W. D. Ellis (Ed. & Trans.),A source book of Gestalt psychology
(pp. 71–88). London: Routledge. (Original work published 1923)
Wheatstone, C. (1938). Contributions to the physiology of vision: I.
On some remarkable and hitherto unobserved phenomena of
binocular vision. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Soci-
ety of London, 128,371–394.
Wraga, M. (1999). The role of eye height in perceiving affordances
and object dimensions.Perception & Psychophysics, 61,490–507.
Wraga, M., Creem, S. H., & Proffitt, D. R. (2000). Perception-action
dissociations of a walkable Müller-Lyer configuration.Psycho-
logical Science, 11,239–243.
Yang, T. L., Dixon, M. W., & Proffitt, D. R. (1999). Seeing big
things: Overestimation of heights is greater for real objects than
for objects in pictures. Perception, 28,445–467.
Yonas, A., Bechtold, A. G., Frankel, D., Gordon, F. R., McRoberts,
G., Norcia, A., & Sternfels, S. (1977). Development of sensitiv-
ity to information for impending collisions. Perception & Psy-
chophysics, 21,97–104.
Yonas, A., Craton, L. G., & Thompson, W. B. (1987). Relative
motion: Kinetic information for the order of depth at an edge.
Perception & Psychophysics, 41,53–59.
Young, M. J., Landy, M. S., & Maloney, L. T. (1993). A perturbation
analysis of depth perception from combinations of texture and
motion cues. Vision Research, 33,2685–2696.
Free download pdf