Handbook of Psychology, Volume 4: Experimental Psychology

(Axel Boer) #1
References 313

De Jagger, J. J. (1970). Reaction time and mental processes
(J. Brozek & M. S. Sibinga, Translators). Nieuwkoop, The
Netherlands: B. De Graf. (Original work published 1865)
De Jong, R., Coles, M. G. H., Logan, G. D., & Gratton, G. (1990).
Searching for the point of no return: The control of response
processes in speeded choice reaction performance. Journal of
Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance,
16,164–182.
De Jong, R., Liang, C.-C., & Lauber, E. (1994). Conditional and
unconditional automaticity: A dual-process model of effects of
spatial stimulus-response compatibility. Journal of Experimental
Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20,731–750.
DeSchepper, B., & Treisman, A. (1996). Visual memory for novel
shapes: Implicit coding without attention.Journal of Experimen-
tal Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22,27–47.
Donders, F. C. (1969). On the speed of mental processes. In W. G.
Koster (Ed.), Acta Psychologica, 30: Vol. 2. Attention and Per-
formance(pp. 412–431). Amsterdam: North-Holland. (Original
work published 1868)
Dosher, B. A. (1979). Empirical approaches to information process-
ing: Speed-accuracy tradeoff functions or reaction time—A
reply. Acta Psychologica, 43,347–359.
Duncan, J. (1977). Response selection rules in spatial choice reac-
tion tasks. In S. Dornic (Ed.). Attention and performance: Vol. 6
(pp. 49–71). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Duncan, J. (1979). Divided attention: The whole is more than the
sum of the parts. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human
Perception and Performance, 5,216–228.
Durgin, F. H. (2000). The reverse Stroop effect. Psychonomic
Bulletin & Review, 7,121–125.
Dutta, A., & Proctor, R. W. (1992). Persistence of stimulus-response
compatibility effects with extended practice. Journal of Exper-
imental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18,
801–809.
Eimer, M. (1998). The lateralized readiness potential as an on-line
measure of central response activation processes. Behavior
Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 30,146–156.
Eriksen, B. A., & Eriksen, C. W. (1974). Effects of noise letters upon
the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task. Perception
& Psychophysics, 16,143–149.
Eriksen, C. W., & Schultz, C. W. (1979). Information processing in
visual search: A continuous flow conception and experimental
results.Perception & Psychophysics, 25,249–263.
Falkenstein, M., Hohnsbein, J., & Hoormann, J. (1994). Effects of
choice complexity on different subcomponents of the late posi-
tive complex of the event-related potential. Electoencephalogra-
phy and Clinical Neurophysiology, 92,148–160.
Fitts, P. M., & Deininger, R. L. (1954). S-R compatibility: Corre-
spondence among paired elements within stimulus and response
codes.Journal of Experimental Psychology, 48,483–492.
Fitts, P. M., & Posner, M. I. (1967). Human performance. Belmont,
CA: Brooks/Cole.


Fitts, P. M., & Seeger, C. M. (1953). S-R compatibility: Spatial
characteristics of stimulus and response codes. Journal of
Experimental Psychology, 46,199–210.
Fox, E. (1995). Negative priming from ignored distractors in
visual selection: A review. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 2,
145–173.
Gratton, G., Coles, M. G. H., Sirevaag, E. J., Eriksen, C. W., &
Donchin, E. (1988). Pre- and poststimulus activation of response
channels: A psychophysiological analysis.Journal of Experimental
Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 14,331–344.
Guiard, Y. (1983). The lateral coding of rotations: A study of the
Simon effect with wheel-rotation responses. Journal of Motor
Behavior, 15,331–342.
Heathcote, A., Brown, S., & Mewhort, D. J. K. (2000). The power
law repealed: The case for an exponential law of practice.
Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 7,185–207.
Hedge, A., & Marsh, N. W. A. (1975). The effect of irrelevant spatial
correspondences on two-choice reaction time. Acta Psycho-
logica, 39,427–439.
Heister, G., Schroeder-Heister, P., & Ehrenstein, W. H. (1990).
Spatial coding and spatio-anatomical mapping: Evidence for a
hierarchical model of spatial stimulus-response compatibility. In
R. W. Proctor & T. G. Reeve (Eds.), Stimulus-response compati-
bility: An integrated perspective(pp. 117–143). Amsterdam:
North-Holland.
Helmholtz, H. (1850). Messungen über den zeitlichen Verlauf der
Zuckung animalischer Muskeln und die Fortpflanzungs-
geschwindigkeit der Reizung in den Nerven [Measurement of the
time course of the muscles and the transmission speed of nerve
stimulation].Archiv für Anatomie und Phsysiologie,276–364.
Helmholtz, H. (1867). Mittheilung, betreffend Versuche über die
Fortpflanzungsgeschwindigkeit der Reizung in den motorischen
Nerven des Menschen, welche Herr. N. Baxt aus Petersburg im
Physiologischen Laboratorium zu Heidelberg ausgeführt hat
[Communication concerning experiments about the transmission
speed of stimulation in the motor nerves of humans, which
Mr. N. Baxt from Petersburg in the Physiology Laboratories at
Heidelberg has performed]. Monatsberichte der Akademie der
Wissenschaften zu Berlin, 29,228–234.
Hick, W. E. (1952). On the rate of gain of information. Quarterly
Journal of Experimental Psychology, 4,11–26.
Hoffman, E. R. (1990a). Strength of component principles deter-
mining direction-of-turn stereotypes for horizontally moving
displays.Proceedings of the Human Factors Society 34th Annual
Meeting,457–461.
Hoffman, E. R. (1990b). Strength of component principles for
direction-of-turn stereotypes of three-dimensional display/control
arrangements.Proceedings of the Human Factors Society 34th
Annual Meeting,462–466.
Hommel, B. (1993a). Inverting the Simon effect with intention:
Determinants of detection and extent of effects of irrelevant spa-
tial information. Psychological Research, 55,270–279.
Free download pdf