Handbook of Psychology, Volume 4: Experimental Psychology

(Axel Boer) #1

58 Motivation


Hirsch, E., & Collier, G. (1974). The ecological determinants of re-
inforcement in the guinea pig. Physiology & Behavior, 12,
239–249.
Hogan, J. A. (1973a). Development of food recognition in young
chicks: Vol. 1. Maturation and nutrition. Journal of Compara-
tive & Physiological Psychology, 83,355–366.
Hogan, J. A. (1973b). Development of food recognition in young
chicks: Vol. 2. Learned association over long delays. Journal of
Comparative & Physiological Psychology, 83,367–373.
Hogan, J. A. (1977). Development of food recognition in young
chicks: Vol. 4. Associative and nonassociative effects of experi-
ence.Journal of Comparative & Physiological Psychology, 91,
839–850.
Holland, P. C. (1981). Acquisition of representation-mediated con-
ditioned food aversions. Learning & Motivation, 12,1–18.
Hollis, K. L., Cadieux, E. L., & Colbert, M. M. (1989). The biolog-
ical function of Pavlovian conditioning: A mechanism for mating
success in the blue gourami (Trichogaster trichopterus).Journal
of Comparative Psychology, 103,115–121.
Hollis, K. L., Pharr, V. L., Dumas, M. J., Britton, G.B., & Field, J.
(1997). Classical conditioning provides paternity advantage
for territorial male blue gouramis (Trichogaster trichopterus).
Journal of Comparative Psychology, 111,219–225.
Howard, T. C. (1962). Conditioned temperature drive in rats. Psy-
chological Reports, 10,371–373.
Hull, C. L. (1943). Principles of behavior: An introduction to be-
havior theory. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
Hull, D. (1973). Thermoregulation in young mammals. In C. G.
Whittow (Ed.), Comparative physiology of thermoregulation.
(Vol. 3, pp. 167–200). New York: Academic Press.
Jacobs, W. J., & LoLordo, V. M. (1980). Constraints on Pavlovian
aversive conditioning: Implications for avoidance learning in the
rat.Learning & Motivation, 11,427–455.
Johnson, D. F., & Collier, G. (1989). Patch choice and meal size of
foraging rats as a function of the profitability of food. Animal
Behaviour, 38,285–297.
Kapp, B. S., Frysinger, R. C., Gallagher, M., & Haselton, J. R.
(1979) Amygdala central nucleus lesions: Effect on heart rate
conditioning in the rabbit.Physiology & Behavior, 23,1109–
1117.
Kaufmann, L. W., Collier, G., Hill, W., & Collins, K. (1980). Meal
cost and meal patterns in an uncaged domestic cat. Physiology &
Behavior, 25,135–137.
Kluver, H., & Bucy, P. C. (1939). Preliminary analysis of the tem-
poral lobes in monkeys. Biological Psychiatry, 42,461–471.
Konorski, J. (1967). Integrative activity of the brain: An interdisci-
plinary approach.Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Le Magnen, J. (1992). Neurobiology of feeding and nutrition.New
York: Academic Press.
Le Magnen, J., & Devos, M. (1980). Parameters of the meal pattern
in rats: their assessment and physiological significance.Neuro-
science & Biobehavioral Reviews, 4,1–11.


Lester, L. S., & Fanselow, M. S. (1985). Exposure to a cat produces
opioid analgesia in rats. Behavioral Neuroscience, 99,756–759.
Leyhausen, P. (1979). Cat behavior: The predatory and social
behavior of domestic and wild cats.New York: Garland.
Lorenz, K. (1937). The establishment of the instinct concept. Die
Naturwissenschaften, 25,280–300.
Maren, S. (1998). Overtraining does not mitigate contextual fear
conditioning deficits produced by neurotoxic lesions of the baso-
lateral amygdala. Journal of Neuroscience, 18,3088–3097.
Martin, L. T., & Alberts, J. R. (1982). Associative learning in neona-
tal rats revealed by cardiac response patterns. Journal of Com-
parative & Physiological Psychology, 96,668–675.
Mineka, S., & Cook, M. (1988). Social learning and the acquisition
of snake fear in monkeys. In T. R. Zentall & B. G. Galef, Jr.
(Eds.),Social learning: Psychological and biological perspec-
tives(pp. 51–73). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Modaresi, H. A. (1982). Defensive behavior of the rat in a shock
prod situation: Effects of the subjects location on preference.
Animal Learning and Behavior, 10,97–102.
Mook, D. G. (1987). Motivation: the organization of action.New
York: W. W. Norton.
Morato, S., Johnson, D. F., & Collier, G. (1995). Feeding patterns of
rats when food-access cost is alternately low and high. Physiol-
ogy & Behavior, 57,21–26.
Morris, D. (1958). The reproductive behavior of the ten-spined
stickleback (Pygosteus pungitius L.).Behaviour, 61,1–154.
Mowrer, O. H., & Lamoreaux, R. R. (1946). Fear as an intervening
variable in avoidance conditioning. Journal of Comparative
Psychology, 39,29–50.
Nader, K., Schafe, G. E., & LeDoux, J. E. (2000). Fear memories
require protein synthesis in the amygdala for reconsolidation
after retrieval. Nature, 406,722–726.
Nakayama, T., Hammel, H. T., Hardy, J. D., & Eisenman, J. S.
(1963). Thermal stimulation of electrical single unit of the pre-
optic region. American Journal of Physiology, 204,1122.
Panksepp, J. (1974). Hypothalamic regulation of energy balance and
feeding behavior. Federation Proceedings, 33,1150–1165.
Pavlov, I. P. (1927). Conditioned reflexes.Oxford: Oxford Univer-
sity Press.
Pfaff, D. W., & Pfaffman, C. (1969). Behavioral and electrophysio-
logical responses of male rats to female rat urine odors. In C.
Pfaffmann (Ed.), Olfaction and taste (pp. 258–267). New York:
Rockefeller University Press.
Pinel, J. P., & Mana, M. (1989). Adaptive interactions of rats with
dangerous inanimate objects: Support for a cognitive theory of
defensive behavior. In R. J. Blanchard & P. F. Brain (Eds.), Etho-
experimental approaches to the study of behavior(pp. 137–150).
Norwell, MA: Kluwer.
Pinel, J. P., Mana, M. J., & Ward, J. A. (1989). Stretched-approach
sequences directed at a localized shock source by Rattus
norvegicus. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 103,140–148.
Free download pdf