Handbook of Psychology, Volume 4: Experimental Psychology

(Axel Boer) #1

64 Mood, Cognition, and Memory


thoughts, and behaviors to become more mood congruent
(Forgas, 1995). However, in order for such infusion effects
to occur, it is necessary for people to adopt an open, elabo-
rate information-processing strategy that facilitates the
incidental use of affectively primed memories and informa-
tion. Thus, the nature and extent of affective influences on
cognition should largely depend on what kind of information-
processing strategy people employ in a particular situation.
Later we will review the empirical evidence for this predic-
tion and describe an integrative theory that emphasizes the
role of information-processing strategies in moderating mood
congruence.


Inferential Accounts


Several theorists maintain that many manifestations of mood
congruence can be readily explained in terms other than
affect priming. Chief among these alternative accounts is the
affect-as-information (AAI) model advanced by Schwarz and
Clore (1983, 1988). This model suggests that “rather than
computing a judgment on the basis of recalled features of a
target, individuals may... ask themselves: ‘how do I feel
about it? [and] in doing so, they may mistake feelings due to
a pre-existing [sic] state as a reaction to the target” (Schwarz,
1990, p. 529). Thus, the model implies that mood congruence
in judgments is due to an inferential error, as people misat-
tribute a preexisting affective state to a judgmental target.
The AAI model incorporates ideas from at least three past
research traditions. First, the predictions of the model are often
indistinguishable from earlier conditioning research by Clore
and Byrne (1974). Whereas the conditioning account empha-
sized blind temporal and spatial contiguity as responsible for
linking affect to judgments, the AAI model, rather less parsi-
moniously, posits an internal inferential process as producing
the same effects (see Berkowitz et al., 2000). A second tradi-
tion that informs the AAI model comes from research on mis-
attribution, according to which judgments are often inferred
on the basis of salient but irrelevant cues: in this case, affective
state. Thus, the AAI model also predicts that only previously
unattributed affect can produce mood congruence. Finally, the
model also shows some affinity with research on judgmental
heuristics (see the chapter by Wallsten & Budescu in this vol-
ume), in the sense that affective states are thought to function
as heuristic cues in informing people’s judgments.
Again, these effects are not universal. Typically, people
rely on affect as a heuristic cue only when “the task is of
little personal relevance, when little other information is
available, when problems are too complex to be solved sys-
tematically, and when time or attentional resources are lim-
ited” (Fiedler, 2001, p. 175). For example, some of the


earliest and still most compelling evidence for the AAI
model came from an experiment (Schwarz & Clore, 1983)
that involved telephoning respondents and asking them un-
expected and unfamiliar questions. In this situation, subjects
have little personal interest or involvement in responding to
a stranger, and they have neither the motivation, time, nor
cognitive resources to engage in extensive processing. Rely-
ing on prevailing affect to infer a response seems a reason-
able strategy under such circumstances. In a different but
related case, Forgas and Moylan (1987) asked almost 1,000
people to complete an attitude survey on the sidewalk out-
side a cinema in which they had just watched either a happy
or a sad film. The results showed strong mood congruence:
Happy theatergoers gave much more positive responses than
did their sad counterparts. In this situation, as in the study
by Schwarz and Clore (1983), respondents presumably had
little time, motivation, or capacity to engage in elaborate
processing, and hence they may well have relied on their
temporary affect as a heuristic cue to infer a reaction.
On the negative side, the AAI model has some serious
shortcomings. First, although the model is applicable to
mood congruence in evaluative judgments, it has difficulty
accounting for the infusion of affect into other cognitive
processes, including attention, learning, and memory. Also,
it is sometimes claimed (e.g., Clore et al., 2001; Schwarz &
Clore, 1988) that the model is supported by the finding that
mood congruence can be eliminated by calling the subjects’
attention to the true source of their mood, thereby minimizing
the possibility of an affect misattribution. This claim is dubi-
ous, as we know that mood congruence due to affect-priming
mechanisms can similarly be reversed by instructing subjects
to focus on their internal states (Berkowitz et al., 2000).
Moreover, Martin (2000) has argued that the informational
value of affective states cannot be regarded as “given” and
permanent, but instead depends on the situational context.
Thus, positive affect may signal that a positive response
is appropriate if the setting happens to be, say, a wedding,
but the same mood may have a different meaning at a funeral.
The AAI model also has nothing to say about how cues other
than affect (such as memories, features of the stimulus, etc.)
can enter into a judgment. In that sense, AAI is really a the-
ory of nonjudgment or aborted judgment, rather than a theory
of judgment. It now appears that in most realistic cognitive
tasks, affect priming rather than the affect-as-information is
the main mechanism producing mood congruence.

Processing Consequences of Moods

In addition to influencing whatpeople think, moods may
also influence the process of cognition, that is, howpeople
Free download pdf